首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   27篇
  免费   2篇
化学   7篇
力学   1篇
数学   1篇
物理学   20篇
  2016年   2篇
  2014年   1篇
  2011年   3篇
  2010年   2篇
  2009年   3篇
  2007年   1篇
  2005年   1篇
  2004年   2篇
  2000年   2篇
  1999年   2篇
  1991年   1篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   2篇
  1976年   1篇
排序方式: 共有29条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
Perceptual integration of vibrotactile and auditory sinusoidal tone pulses was studied in detection experiments as a function of stimulation frequency. Vibrotactile stimuli were delivered through a single channel vibrator to the left middle fingertip. Auditory stimuli were presented diotically through headphones in a background of 50 dB sound pressure level broadband noise. Detection performance for combined auditory-tactile presentations was measured using stimulus levels that yielded 63% to 77% correct unimodal performance. In Experiment 1, the vibrotactile stimulus was 250 Hz and the auditory stimulus varied between 125 and 2000 Hz. In Experiment 2, the auditory stimulus was 250 Hz and the tactile stimulus varied between 50 and 400 Hz. In Experiment 3, the auditory and tactile stimuli were always equal in frequency and ranged from 50 to 400 Hz. The highest rates of detection for the combined-modality stimulus were obtained when stimulating frequencies in the two modalities were equal or closely spaced (and within the Pacinian range). Combined-modality detection for closely spaced frequencies was generally consistent with an algebraic sum model of perceptual integration; wider-frequency spacings were generally better fit by a Pythagorean sum model. Thus, perceptual integration of auditory and tactile stimuli at near-threshold levels appears to depend both on absolute frequency and relative frequency of stimulation within each modality.  相似文献   
3.
Some typical hypervalent molecules, SF4, PF5, and ClF3, as well as precursors SF (4Σ? state) and SF2 (3B1 state), are studied by means of the breathing‐orbital valence bond (BOVB) method, chosen for its capability of combining compactness with accuracy of energetics. A unique feature of this study is that for the first time, the method used to gain insight into the bonding modes is the same as that used to calculate the bonding energies, so as to guarantee that the qualitative picture obtained captures the essential physics of the bonding system. The 4Σ? state of SF is shown to be bonded by a three‐electron σ bond assisted by strong π back‐donation of dynamic nature. The linear 3B1 state of SF2, as well as the ground states of SF4, PF5 and ClF3, are described in terms of four VB structures that all have significant weights in the range 0.17–0.31, with exceptionally large resonance energies arising from their mixing. It is concluded that the bonding mode of these hypervalent species and isoelectronic ones complies with Coulson’s version of the Rundle–Pimentel model, but assisted by charge‐shift bonding. The conditions for hypervalence to occur are stated.  相似文献   
4.
Principal-component amplitude compression for the hearing impaired   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Principal-component amplitude compression, a means for matching speech to the reduced dynamic range in sensorineural hearing impairments, is a multiband approach aimed at preserving details of spectral shape while reducing overall level variation. The effect of compression has been studied for the first and second principal components (PC1 an PC2) of the short-term speech spectrum, which are roughly representative of overall level and spectral tilt, respectively. Compression of PC1 roughly equalizes consonant and vowel levels while compression of PC2 provides time-varying high-frequency emphasis. The effect on speech intelligibility of sensorineural hearing-impaired listeners of two principal-component compression system implementations, compression of PC1 and compression of both PC1 and PC2, was compared to that of linear amplification (LA), independent compression of multiple bands (MBC), and wideband compression (WC). Results indicate that compression of overall level as provided by compression of PC1 and WC improved intelligibility relative to LA over a 10- to 15-dB range of input levels. While MBC was beneficial in some cases, it did not provide higher intelligibility than WC. Compression of PC2 did not benefit but rather degraded performance relative to LA. Error analyses and band-level measurements indicate that the highest intelligibility is obtained when audibility is improved and the relative spectral shapes of different speech sounds are preserved.  相似文献   
5.
The presence of a peak centered near m/z 2862, observed for the first time for the caged dodecatungstate radical-anion, [W12O41]−·, enables distinguishing WO2 from WO3 by Laser Desorption Ionization mass spectrometry (LDI-MS). In addition to WO2, laser irradiation of dry deposits made from aqueous ammonium paratungstate, and calcium and lead orthotungstate also produce the [W12O41]−·. In contrast, spectra recorded from deposits made from aqueous Na2WO4, sodium metatungstate, and WO3, or non-aqueous calcium and lead orthotungstate, and ammonium paratungstate, failed to show the m/z 2862 peak cluster. These observations support the hypothesis that polycondensation reactions to form [W12O41]−· occur solely in the presence of water. Although dry spots are irradiated for ionization, the solvent used for sample preparation plays an important role on the chemical composition endowed to ions detected. For example, the m/z 2862 peak seen from deposits made from aqueous ammonium paratungstate, and calcium and lead orthotungstate, is absent in the spectra recorded either from pristine deposits or those derived from solutions made with organic solvents such as acetonitrile or ethanol.  相似文献   
6.
A functional simulation of hearing loss was evaluated in its ability to reproduce the temporal masking functions for eight listeners with mild to severe sensorineural hearing loss. Each audiometric loss was simulated in a group of age-matched normal-hearing listeners through a combination of spectrally-shaped masking noise and multi-band expansion. Temporal-masking functions were obtained in both groups of listeners using a forward-masking paradigm in which the level of a 110-ms masker required to just mask a 10-ms fixed-level probe (5-10 dB SL) was measured as a function of the time delay between the masker offset and probe onset. At each of four probe frequencies (500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz), temporal-masking functions were obtained using maskers that were 0.55, 1.0, and 1.15 times the probe frequency. The slopes and y-intercepts of the masking functions were not significantly different for listeners with real and simulated hearing loss. The y-intercepts were positively correlated with level of hearing loss while the slopes were negatively correlated. The ratio of the slopes obtained with the low-frequency maskers relative to the on-frequency maskers was similar for both groups of listeners and indicated a smaller compressive effect than that observed in normal-hearing listeners.  相似文献   
7.
8.
Multiple periodic solutions for a nonlinear suspension bridge equation   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
We investigate nonlinear oscillations in a fourth-order partialdifferential equation which models a suspension bridge. Previouswork establishes multiple periodic solutions when a parameterexceeds a certain eigenvalue. In this paper, we use Leray-Schauderdegree theory to prove that if the parameter is increased further,beyond a second eigenvalue, then additional solutions are created.  相似文献   
9.
Identification experiments using 13 tone-pulse stimuli (1000 Hz, 500 ms) spaced by equal decibel increments from 42 to 90 dB SPL were conducted under three conditions: all intensities equally likely (denoted Q), the middle intensity presented on roughly 1/3 of the trials (M), and each of the extreme intensities presented on roughly 1/5 of the trials (E). Experiments were performed both with and without feedback, and results were analyzed in terms of sensitivity and bias. Similar results were obtained with and without feedback: listeners generally shifted biases in the M and E conditions relative to the Q condition to increase percent-correct scores, but there were only minor changes in sensitivity from condition to condition. However, sensitivities were generally higher and biases more optimal when feedback was available. The results on sensitivity are consistent with those associated with payoff variation [Lippmann et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 59, 129-134 (1976)], but are inconsistent with those reported for presentation probability variation in tone frequency identification [Cuddy et al., J. Exp. Psychol. 100, 218-220 (1973)]. In an auxilliary experiment concerned with tone frequency identification, results consistent with those obtained for intensity, but inconsistent with those reported by Cuddy et al., were obtained.  相似文献   
10.
The loudness of auditory (A), tactile (T), and auditory-tactile (A+T) stimuli was measured at supra-threshold levels. Auditory stimuli were pure tones presented binaurally through headphones; tactile stimuli were sinusoids delivered through a single-channel vibrator to the left middle fingertip. All stimuli were presented together with a broadband auditory noise. The A and T stimuli were presented at levels that were matched in loudness to that of the 200-Hz auditory tone at 25 dB sensation level. The 200-Hz auditory tone was then matched in loudness to various combinations of auditory and tactile stimuli (A+T), and purely auditory stimuli (A+A). The results indicate that the matched intensity of the 200-Hz auditory tone is less when the A+T and A+A stimuli are close together in frequency than when they are separated by an octave or more. This suggests that A+T integration may operate in a manner similar to that found in auditory critical band studies, further supporting a strong frequency relationship between the auditory and somatosensory systems.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号