首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   5813篇
  免费   274篇
  国内免费   41篇
化学   3867篇
晶体学   28篇
力学   157篇
数学   889篇
物理学   1187篇
  2023年   52篇
  2022年   78篇
  2021年   102篇
  2020年   160篇
  2019年   144篇
  2018年   103篇
  2017年   78篇
  2016年   234篇
  2015年   178篇
  2014年   231篇
  2013年   319篇
  2012年   468篇
  2011年   551篇
  2010年   286篇
  2009年   198篇
  2008年   380篇
  2007年   382篇
  2006年   365篇
  2005年   329篇
  2004年   238篇
  2003年   174篇
  2002年   139篇
  2001年   79篇
  2000年   62篇
  1999年   41篇
  1998年   40篇
  1997年   31篇
  1996年   55篇
  1995年   58篇
  1994年   40篇
  1993年   52篇
  1992年   37篇
  1991年   26篇
  1990年   21篇
  1989年   22篇
  1988年   15篇
  1987年   15篇
  1986年   18篇
  1985年   31篇
  1984年   18篇
  1983年   20篇
  1982年   25篇
  1981年   16篇
  1980年   19篇
  1979年   26篇
  1978年   16篇
  1977年   24篇
  1976年   26篇
  1975年   18篇
  1973年   37篇
排序方式: 共有6128条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
Both English and Japanese have two voiceless sibilant fricatives, an anterior fricative /s/ contrasting with a more posterior fricative /∫/. When children acquire sibilant fricatives, English children typically substitute [s] for /∫/, whereas Japanese children typically substitute [∫] for /s/. This study examined English- and Japanese-speaking adults' perception of children's productions of voiceless sibilant fricatives to investigate whether the apparent asymmetry in the acquisition of voiceless sibilant fricatives reported previously in the two languages was due in part to how adults perceive children's speech. The results of this study show that adult speakers of English and Japanese weighed acoustic parameters differently when identifying fricatives produced by children and that these differences explain, in part, the apparent cross-language asymmetry in fricative acquisition. This study shows that generalizations about universal and language-specific patterns in speech-sound development cannot be determined without considering all sources of variation including speech perception.  相似文献   
82.
Photoluminescence and resonant Raman scattering have been studied in GaAs under hydrostatic stress up to 72 kbar. From the pressure dependence of the photoluminescence intensity the pressure coefficient of the X6 conduction band minimum in GaAs has been determined. The pressure dependence of zone edge phonon energies in GaAs is also reported.  相似文献   
83.
Let p be a prime and let b be a positive integer. If a (v, k, λ, n) difference set D of order n = p b exists in an abelian group with cyclic Sylow p-subgroup S, then \({p\in\{2,3\}}\) and |S| = p. Furthermore, either p = 2 and vλ ≡ 2 (mod 4) or the parameters of D belong to one of four families explicitly determined in our main theorem.  相似文献   
84.
Finitary Markov processes are described in G. Morvai and B. Weiss, Prediction for discrete time series, Probability Theory and Related Fields 132 (2005), 1–12. The transition functions of finitary Markov processes are residually locally constant g-functions that can be extended by continuity to their maximal domain of definition. The study of their associated symbolic dynamics leads one to the D-shifts as introduced in W. Krieger, On g-functions for subshifts, Institute of Mathematical Statistics Lecture Notes-Monograph Series, Vol. 48, Dynamics & Stochastics, arXiv:math.DS/0608259, (2006), 306–316, We study the phenomena that can arise in residually locally constant and locally constant maximally defined g-functions on D-shifts, Markov shifts and synchronizing systems with respect to future measures and g-measures  相似文献   
85.
We report on a measurement of the mass dependence of the forward-backward charge asymmetry, A(FB), and production cross section d sigma/dM for e(+)e(-) pairs with mass M(ee)>40 GeV/c(2). The data sample consists of 108 pb(-1) of pp macro collisions at square root of s = 1.8 TeV taken by the Collider Detector at Fermilab during 1992-1995. The measured asymmetry and d sigma/dM are compared with the predictions of the standard model and a model with an extra Z' gauge boson.  相似文献   
86.
Bayesian inference for the power law process   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
The power law process has been used to model reliability growth, software reliability and the failure times of repairable systems. This article reviews and further develops Bayesian inference for such a process. The Bayesian approach provides a unified methodology for dealing with both time and failure truncated data. As well as looking at the posterior densities of the parameters of the power law process, inference for the expected number of failures and the probability of no failures in some given time interval is discussed. Aspects of the prediction problem are examined. The results are illustrated with two data examples.  相似文献   
87.
光纤光栅腐蚀传感器   总被引:4,自引:2,他引:4  
江毅  严云 《光子学报》2006,35(1):96-99
提出了一种测量钢筋腐蚀的新型传感器.这种传感器将光纤光栅拉伸后固定在圆形钢筋的表面,在钢筋被腐蚀后,光纤光栅所受到的拉伸应变将被释放,光纤光栅的反射光波长发生变化,通过测量光纤光栅的波长就可以测得钢筋腐蚀程度.这种传感器测量准确度优于±0.1 μm,测量范围约12 μm,可用于混凝土结构中钢筋腐蚀的早期监测.  相似文献   
88.
We have performed a search for B(s)(0) --> micro(+) micro(-) and B(0) --> micro(+) micro(-) decays in pp collisions at square root s = 1.96 TeV using 2 fb(-1) of integrated luminosity collected by the CDF II detector at the Fermilab Tevatron Collider. The observed number of B(s)(0) and B0 candidates is consistent with background expectations. The resulting upper limits on the branching fractions are B(B(s)0) --> micro(+) micro(-)) <5.8 x 10(-8) and B(B(0) --> micro(+) micro(-))<1.8 x 10(-8) at 95% C.L.  相似文献   
89.

Background

Tone languages such as Thai and Mandarin Chinese use differences in fundamental frequency (F0, pitch) to distinguish lexical meaning. Previous behavioral studies have shown that native speakers of a non-tone language have difficulty discriminating among tone contrasts and are sensitive to different F0 dimensions than speakers of a tone language. The aim of the present ERP study was to investigate the effect of language background and training on the non-attentive processing of lexical tones. EEG was recorded from 12 adult native speakers of Mandarin Chinese, 12 native speakers of American English, and 11 Thai speakers while they were watching a movie and were presented with multiple tokens of low-falling, mid-level and high-rising Thai lexical tones. High-rising or low-falling tokens were presented as deviants among mid-level standard tokens, and vice versa. EEG data and data from a behavioral discrimination task were collected before and after a two-day perceptual categorization training task.

Results

Behavioral discrimination improved after training in both the Chinese and the English groups. Low-falling tone deviants versus standards elicited a mismatch negativity (MMN) in all language groups. Before, but not after training, the English speakers showed a larger MMN compared to the Chinese, even though English speakers performed worst in the behavioral tasks. The MMN was followed by a late negativity, which became smaller with improved discrimination. The High-rising deviants versus standards elicited a late negativity, which was left-lateralized only in the English and Chinese groups.

Conclusion

Results showed that native speakers of English, Chinese and Thai recruited largely similar mechanisms when non-attentively processing Thai lexical tones. However, native Thai speakers differed from the Chinese and English speakers with respect to the processing of late F0 contour differences (high-rising versus mid-level tones). In addition, native speakers of a non-tone language (English) were initially more sensitive to F0 onset differences (low-falling versus mid-level contrast), which was suppressed as a result of training. This result converges with results from previous behavioral studies and supports the view that attentive as well as non-attentive processing of F0 contrasts is affected by language background, but is malleable even in adult learners.  相似文献   
90.
Optical detection of the frequency-dependent magnetic relaxation signal is used to monitor the binding of biological molecules to magnetic nanoparticles in a ferrofluid. Biological binding reactions cause changes in the magnetic relaxation signal due to an increase in the average hydrodynamic diameter of the nanoparticles. To allow the relaxation signal to be detected in dilute ferrofluids, measurements are made using a balanced photodetector, resulting in a 25 μV/√Hz noise floor, within 50% of the theoretical limit imposed by photon shot noise. Measurements of a ferrofluid composed of magnetite nanoparticles coated with anti-IgG antibodies show that the average hydrodynamic diameter increases from 115.2 to 125.4 nm after reaction with IgG.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号