首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 234 毫秒
1.
Two experiments concerning the effects of masking noise on the gerbil brain-stem auditory-evoked response (BAER) are reported. Experiment 1 evaluated the effects of broadband masking noise on the BAER obtained to condensation and rarefaction clicks. With increasing noise level, there was an increase in BAER peak latencies, an increase in the i-v interval, and a decrease in peak amplitudes. Experiment 2 evaluated the effects of high-pass masking noise on the BAER obtained to condensation and rarefaction clicks. Both high-pass responses and derived-band responses were evaluated. For high-pass responses, with decreasing masker cutoff frequency, there was an increase in BAER peak latencies, a decrease in the i-v interval, and a decrease in peak amplitudes. For derived-band responses, with decreasing derived-band frequency, there was an increase in peak latencies and a decrease in the i-v interval. A comparison of wave i and wave v amplitudes across derived-band frequency demonstrates a greater contribution of high-frequency cochlear regions to wave i than wave v. Small, insignificant, effects of click polarity on BAER peak amplitudes were observed. These trends were in the direction seen in a companion paper [R. Burkard and H. F. Voigt, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 2514-2525 (1989)] and were, in general, reduced by the presence of broadband or high-pass maskers.  相似文献   

2.
Two experiments were performed that evaluated the effects of ipsilateral-direct broadband noise maskers on the gerbil brain-stem auditory-evoked response (BAER) to click stimuli. In experiment 1, clicks were presented at 27 Hz at levels including 70, 80, 90, and 100 dB pSPL. Noise conditions included a no-noise control, and included noise levels varying in 10-dB increments from 20 dB SPL to a maximum noise level of 50, 60, 70, and 80 dB SPL for click levels of 70, 80, 90, and 100 dB pSPL, respectively. Gerbil BAER peaks were labeled with small roman numerals to distinguish them from human BAER peaks. The dependent variables included waves i and v latencies and amplitudes. Peak latencies increased and peak amplitudes decreased with decreasing click level and increasing noise level. To a first approximation, peak latencies and amplitudes showed changes with increasing noise level that were similar across click level. With increasing click level, there was little or no effect on the i-v interval. There was an increase in the i-v interval with increasing noise level. In experiment 2, click level was held constant at 90 dB pSPL, and click rates included 15, 40, 65, and 90 Hz. For each click rate, noise conditions included a no-noise control, and noise levels included 20, 30, 40, 50, 60, and 70 dB SPL. With increasing click rate and noise level, there was an increase in peak latencies, an increase in the i-v interval, and a decrease in peak amplitudes. The magnitude of peak latency and amplitude shifts with increasing click rate was dependent on noise level. Specifically, the magnitude of rate-dependent changes decreased with increasing level of broadband noise. These data are compared to human BAER experiments, and are found to be in fundamental agreement.  相似文献   

3.
The effects of broadband noise on the brain-stem auditory evoked response (BAER) are reported for two experiments. Experiment 1 used a high-pass subtractive-masking technique and covaried derived bandwidth and continuous broadband noise level. Comparison of responses to half-octave wide derived bands in the presence of within-band noise showed that wave V latency changes were greater than could be explained on the basis of shifts in the cochlear region responsible for generating the response. The magnitude of within-band noise-induced wave V latency shift was independent of the frequency separation of the masker cutoffs. In experiment 2 the effects of noise level and rate on waves I, III, and V of the BAER were evaluated. Peak latencies increased and peak amplitudes decreased with increasing noise level and rate. Higher noise levels and rates produced an increased central (I-V) conduction time in which the wave III-V increase was greater than the wave I-III increase. Together, these results are most consistent with the hypothesis that a nonplace, central auditory mechanism produces most of the noise-induced latency shifts in normal-hearing adults.  相似文献   

4.
A series of experiments evaluated the effects of broadband noise (ipsilateral) on wave V of the brainstem auditory evoked response (BAER) elicited by tone bursts or clicks in the presence of high-pass masking noise. Experiment 1 used 1000- and 4000-Hz, 60-dB nHL tone bursts in the presence of broadband noise. With increasing noise level, wave V latency shift was greater for the 1000-Hz tone bursts, while amplitude decrements were similar for both tone-burst frequencies. Experiment 2 varied high-pass masker cutoff frequency and the level of subtotal masking in the presence of 50-dB nHL clicks. The effects of subtotal masking on wave V (increase in latency and decrease in amplitude) increased with increasing derived-band frequency. Experiment 3 covaried high-pass masker cutoff frequency and subtotal masking level for 1000- and 4000-Hz tone-burst stimuli. The effect of subtotal masking on wave V latency was reduced for both tone-burst frequencies when the response-generating region of the cochlear partition was limited by high-pass maskers. The results of these three experiments suggest that most of the wave V latency shift associated with increasing levels of broadband noise is mediated by a place mechanism when the stimulus is a moderate intensity (60 dB nHL), low-frequency (1000 Hz) tone burst. However, the interpretation of the latency shifts produced by broadband noise for 4000-Hz tone-burst stimuli is made more complex by multiple technical factors discussed herein.  相似文献   

5.
The effects of masking noise on wave V of the brain-stem auditory-evoked response (BAER) obtained to pseudorandom pulse sequences are evaluated in two experiments. In the first experiment, the level of broadband noise was covaried with minimum pulse interval (rate) using maximum length sequence analysis (MLSA). Both increasing noise level and decreasing minimum pulse interval decrease wave V amplitude and increase wave V latency. A nonadditivity of rate and noise level was observed such that, at the shortest interpulse intervals, simultaneous background noise produced virtually no latency change and minimal amplitude change, for the noise levels tested. In a second experiment, high-pass masking was performed to assess the feasibility of derived-band techniques using maximum length sequence analysis (MLSA) and to compare the frequency regions responsible for the BAER using MLSA versus conventional averaging. Results of experiment 2 showed that reliable responses across high-pass masker cutoff frequency could be obtained in normal-hearing listeners. The frequency specificity of the MLSA-based responses was nearly identical to that obtained by conventional averaging, although both amplitude and latency of wave V were affected by the high-pass masker cutoff and minimum pulse interval values. These studies suggest that the neuronal populations and frequency regions responsible for the BAER are virtually the same for MLSA and conventional averaging.  相似文献   

6.
A series of experiments investigated the effects of continuous broadband noise (ipsilateral) on wave V of the click-evoked brainstem auditory evoked response (BAER). In general, a broadband noise masker increases the latency and decreases the amplitude of wave V. Varying both click and noise intensity, it was found that noise levels above about 40 dB SPL increase the latency and decrease the amplitude of wave V, regardless of click intensity. The effects of noise on wave V amplitude appear constant across click intensity, whereas the effects of a constant noise level on wave V latency decrease at higher click intensities. Both masking and adaptation increase wave V latency, but their combined effects are occlusive: rate-induced wave V latency shift decreases in the presence of continuous broadband noise. The clinical and theoretical implications of these findings are discussed.  相似文献   

7.
The additivity of forward masking and repetitive stimulation effects on wave V of the brain-stem auditory evoked response (BAER) was investigated. The effects of repetitive stimulation were evaluated for a stimulus train (called the adaptation series), with a 12.5-ms within-train interclick interval. The forward masker was a 100-ms, 80-dB SPL broadband noise with forward-masker intervals ranging from 12.5-87.5 ms. Forward masking and repetitive stimulation increased the latency of wave V of the BAER. The combined forward masking/adaptation series produced less wave V latency shift than the summed individual effects. Forward masking reduced wave V amplitude at brief forward masker intervals, while repetitive stimulation did not affect wave V amplitude. Wave V amplitude was decreased for the combined forward masking/adaptation series, and the time course of amplitude recovery of the combination was prolonged compared to the forward masking alone condition. The nonadditivity of forward masking and rate effects on wave V latency is similar to that found for repetitive stimulation and simultaneous masking [Burkard and Hecox, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 74, 1204-1213 (1983)]. These findings are consistent with the position that forward masking and rate effects on wave V latency are produced by overlapping mechanisms.  相似文献   

8.
Brainstem electrical responses (BSER) to 60-dB-SL click in noise high passed at various cutoff frequencies separated b 1/2-octave steps were recorded in normal-hearing adult subjects. By applying a derived response technique, narrow-band contributions to the BSER from specific portions of the basilar membrane were revealed. Latencies and amplitudes of the various waves in the derived BSER were recorded. Results indicate that nearly the whole cochlear partition can contribute to the brainstem response. The shifts in latency of waves I, III, and V and amplitude changes of waves I and III as a function of CF appear to be fully comparable to those of the AP. In contrast, the amplitude behavior of wave V as a function of CF is different from waves I and III depending upon frequency range. The discrepency in the behavior of wave V with respect to the earlier waves suggests some sort of neural reorganization at the level where was V is generated. The fact that there are contributions to the brainstem response from apical portions of the cochlea opens the possibility for extending the brainstem technique in assessing the higher cochlear turn function.  相似文献   

9.
Experiments were conducted to assess the contribution of place mechanisms to the effect of rise--fall time on wave V of the human brainstem auditory evoked response (BAER). Noise bursts of 4- and 10-ms duration were presented at various rise-fall times (0, 1, 2, and 5 ms). Subtractive high-pass masking techniques were used to determine the effect of rise time as a function of derived-band frequency. In general, increasing rise time prolonged wave V latency but did not affect amplitude. Rise-time effects did not depend on derived-band frequency and similar effects were seen in the unmasked conditions. In addition, narrowing the derived band did not alter the observed effects on latency and amplitude. Signal envelope showed no effects on traveling wave velocity. These results suggest that place mechanisms contribute little to changes in the BAER associated with rise--fall time.  相似文献   

10.
We recorded normal electrophysiological responses to third-octave filtered clicks from external auditory meatus (EAM) and vertex electrodes referred to coupled earlobe electrodes (forehead ground). From both vertex and EAM, polarity-sensitive responses predominated at low frequencies and exhibited characteristics of both phase-locked neural responses (frequency-following response or FFR) and cochlear microphonics (CM). The FFR-like response predominated at the vertex site and the CM-like response predominated at EAM. At high frequencies, polarity-insensitive responses closely resembled rectangular-pulse click action potentials and brainstem evoked potentials, with clearly defined N1 and V peaks recorded from EAM and vertex, respectively. As frequency was lowered, the N1 and V peak latencies increased, the peaks broadened, and the latency-intensity curves steepened with greater prolongation occurring at lower click intensities. Lowering click frequency also shortened the N1-V interval and caused the plot of N1-V interval versus click intensity to become steeper. Plots of polarity-insensitive response amplitudes and thresholds against frequency revealed a high frequency bias for both N1 and V, but the V "frequency response" was flatter. A possible explanation of the shortened N1-V interval at low click frequencies based on this flatter V "Frequency response" is presented.  相似文献   

11.
Nowadays, it is widely believed that the temporal structure of the auditory nerve fibers' response to sound stimuli plays an important role in auditory perception. An influential hypothesis is that information is extracted from this temporal structure by neural operations akin to an autocorrelation algorithm. The goal of the present work was to test this hypothesis. The stimuli consisted of sequences of unipolar clicks that were high-pass filtered and mixed with low-pass noise so as to exclude spectral cues. In experiment 1, "interfering" clicks were inserted in an otherwise periodic (isochronous) click sequence. Each click belonging to the periodic sequence was followed, after a random portion of the period, by one interfering click. This disrupted the detection of temporal regularity, even when the interfering clicks were 5 dB less intense than the periodic clicks. Experiments 2-4 used click sequences that showed a single peak in their autocorrelation functions. For some sequences, this peak originated from "first-order" temporal regularities, that is from the temporal relations between consecutive clicks. For other sequences, the peak originated instead from "second-order" regularities, relative to nonconsecutive clicks. The detection of second-order regularities appeared to be much more difficult than the detection of comparable first-order regularities. Overall, these results do not tally with the current autocorrelation models of temporal processing. They suggest that the extraction of temporal information from a group of closely spaced spectral components makes no use of time intervals between nonconsecutive peaks of the amplitude envelope.  相似文献   

12.
Collinear light diffraction by three-frequency sound is investigated theoretically. The amplitude distributions of transmitted and diffracted light waves along the cell are calculated for different amplitudes of sound signals. The dependence of the intensity of principal diffraction peaks on the frequency difference between acoustic signal components is studied for different amplitude ratios of these components. It is shown that the character of this dependence for a wave being in synchronism differs substantially from that for two other waves characterized by detuning. The dependence of the amplitudes of principal and parasitic diffraction peaks on the efficiency of acoustooptical interaction is investigated. It is demonstrated that parasitic sideband components in diffracted light can play a considerable role if the diffraction efficiency is sufficiently high and exceeds 80%.  相似文献   

13.
By idealizing combustion or heat addition processes to occur over a short distance in the flow direction it is possible to calculate the amplitude and phase of the disturbances corresponding to small amplitude fluctuations in the heat addition. The fluctuating heat input is assumed to vary sinusoidally with time and with distance along the direction normal to the flow. Pressure waves propagate away from the heat input region upstream and downstream, whilst on the downstream side waves of vorticity and entropy are convected away. Strong resonant peaks in the pressure and vorticity waves are present close to the cut-off condition of the pressure waves in two dimensions. Generally the wave amplitudes tend to be higher when the mean flow velocity into the region is close to sonic and to become smaller as the steady heat input is increased. For a simplified calculation in which the combustion chamber discharges directly into a multi-stage turbine the downstream noise was predominantly due to the interaction of the entropy with the turbine (i.e., “indirect” rather than “direct” combustion noise).  相似文献   

14.
 采用VISAR和X光联合测试技术,利用等厚对称和逆向碰撞法测量了FeMnNi合金高压加卸载历程和相变层裂信息。加载过程中,FeMnNi合金样品发生α→ε相转变,相变波速大于塑性波速,在撞击面上相变波与塑性波合并成单一相变波;卸载过程中,FeMnNi合金样品可能发生了逆相变,形成了除合并相变波在自由面反射中心稀疏波R以外的两道卸载波S1和S2。等厚对称高压加载下,FeMnNi合金样品发生了二次层裂。分析中心稀疏波R、卸载波S1和S2在样品中的传播作用过程,发现样品发生冲击相变和卸载逆转变是导致其等厚对称高压加载下发生二次层裂行为的主要原因。  相似文献   

15.
False killer whale Pseudorca crassidens auditory brainstem responses (ABR) were recorded using a double-click stimulation paradigm specifically measuring the recovery of the second response (to the test click) as a function of the inter-click interval (ICI) at various levels of the conditioning and test click. At all click intensities, the slopes of recovery functions were almost constant: 0.6-0.8 microV per ICI decade. Therefore, even when the conditioning-to-test-click level ratio was kept constant, the duration of recovery was intensity-dependent: The higher intensity the longer the recovery. The conditioning-to-test-click level ratio strongly influenced the recovery time: The higher the ratio, the longer the recovery. The dependence was almost linear using a logarithmic ICI scale with a rate of 25-30 dB per ICI decade. These data were used for modeling the interaction between the emitted click and the echo during echolocation, assuming that the two clicks simulated the transmitted and echo clicks. This simulation showed that partial masking of the echo by the preceding emitted click may explain the independence of echo-response amplitude of target distance. However, the distance range where this mechanism is effective depends on the emitted click level: The higher the level, the greater the range. @ 2007 Acoustical Society of America.  相似文献   

16.
The paper presents a revised view of quantum mechanics centered on the notion (genuine fortuitousness) that the click in a counter is a totally lawless event, which comes by itself. A crucial point is the distinction between events on the spacetime scene and the content of the symbolic algorism. A revised conception of matrix variables emerges, by which such a variable, as part of a whole, does not have a value, under any circumstance. This conception is at variance with that of indeterminate variables. A matrix variable not having a value does not enter spacetime and is not a measurable quantity, but manifests itself by a click in a counter with the remarkable property of having an onset, a beginning, from which the click develops. The individual click with its immense complexity is unique and lawless, even beyond probability. The notion of probability only applies to clicks in low resolution, and the completeness of a probabilistic theory is thereby seen in a new perspective. The genuinely fortuitous click is not produced by the impact of a particle, nor caused by an event in the source prior to the click in the counter. Indeed, there are no particles on the spacetime scene. The theory is thereby liberated from notions, which go with particles having indeterminate variables, and which have given to quantum mechanics the image of an unfathomable theory. With no particle as intermediary, the connection between source and detector is non-local, as is the entire theory, which deals exclusively with distributions of clicks. The locality permeating quantum mechanics is a symbolic one. The wave function enters in the sole role of encoding the probability distributions of clicks. The quest to understand the occurrence of the click in terms of the evolution of the wave function loses its meaning. However, click distributions in low resolution can be analyzed in terms of the connection between click distributions for different sets of counters, as given by the wave function. With increasing resolution, the probabilities, and the wave function, gradually lose significance, whereby the onset remains beyond reach. Thus, the downward path from events on the spacetime scene does not extend beyond the onset. The notion that quantum mechanics deals with particles (or fields) is rooted in the historical evolution but appears unable to accomodate genuine fortuitousness. The latter concept is given its due place by fully accepting the abstract nature of the matrix variables.  相似文献   

17.
Nonlinear processes accompanying the focusing of a microsecond acoustic pulse produced by an electromagnetic source shaped as a spherical segment are investigated. The processes are considered to be far from the boundaries of a liquid, in the absence of cavitation. Detailed measurements of the pressure field by a fiber-optic sensor and high-speed photography of the shock front are performed. The pressure field is found to be determined by the nonlinear effects that occur in the course of the propagation of the initial converging compression wave and an edge rarefaction wave. The peak pressure amplitudes at the focus are 75 and ?42 MPa for the compression and rarefaction waves, respectively, at the maximum voltage of the pulse generator in use. The measured length of the compression wave front is equal to the response time of the sensor (8 ns). The pressure amplitude is shown to be limited by the irregularity of the propagation of a shock wave in the form of Mach’s disk. At the focus, the pressure gradient across the radiator axis reaches 0.5 atm/μm, while the diameter of the focal spot is 2.5±0.2 mm. The focus of the edge rarefaction wave formed due to diffraction is located closer to the radiator than the focus of the compression wave, which may facilitate the study of the biological effect of cavitation independently of the shear motion of the medium.  相似文献   

18.
An analytical investigation of the effect of three-wave resonant interactions with the linearly unstable wave is proposed. We consider the waves in the Kelvin-Helmholtz model, consisting of two fluid layers with different densities and velocities. We suppose that the velocity shear is weakly supercritical, the instability is of the algebraic type, i.e., the amplitude of the unstable wave grows linearly, and the instability occurs within the framework of a single mode. The amplitudes of two other waves taking part in the nonlinear interaction are assumed to be stable. The initial amplitudes of these waves are supposed to be small in comparison with the initial amplitude of the unstable wave. We present an analysis of the system of amplitude equations derived for this case using JWKB-method. As a result, we obtain equations that couple solutions pre- and post-passing the singular point, i.e., the point where the amplitude of the unstable wave has a local minimum. These equations give us the transformation rule of a parameter that characterizes the phase shift between fast and slow waves and defines the behavior of the system. This parameter is constant between two singular points and varies by chance at a singular point. As long as it stays positive, the amplitude of the wave remains limited and performs stochastic oscillations. If this parameter passes over zero, then we leave the region of stabilization and turn out in the region, where the amplitude grows infinitely. Accordingly, the transition to the region of instability happens stochastically. However, if the time interval, when the amplitude remains bounded, is large enough, the proposed scenario can be treated as a partial stabilization of instability.  相似文献   

19.
20.
Development of auditory-evoked potentials in the cat. III. Wave amplitudes   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Amplitudes of auditory-evoked brain stem response (ABR) and late-occurring auditory-evoked potential (AER) components were recorded from kittens between birth and 90 postnatal days. All ABR and AER wave amplitudes increased during the first postnatal month. Wave amplitudes exhibited nonmonotonic growth with increasing age, attaining a maximum at 40-60 days of age, after which amplitudes decreased. Amplitudes of waves originating in the auditory nerve matured somewhat faster than waves originating in the brain stem and forebrain, and the order in which waves reached maturity was roughly the reverse order of the latencies of their peaks. Input-output curves for ABR and AER waves displayed nonmonotonic behavior that varied as a function of postnatal age. Wave amplitudes recorded from adult cats increased between threshold and 70 dB SPL, then decreased between 70 and 100 dB SPL, and rapidly increased above 100 dB SPL. The intensity corresponding to the change from increasing to decreasing amplitudes was higher for younger animals and achieved adult values during the first postnatal month.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号