首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Recently, Klein and Hartmann [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 70, 51-61 (1981)] investigated a new dichotic pitch, called the binaural edge pitch (BEP). They used computer-generated periodic noise signals to generate BEP. In the study presented here, the BEP is investigated in order to evaluate the predictions of the central spectrum theory with regard to this stimulus. Pitch-matching experiments using a nonperiodic BEP stimulus, produced by means of a modulation technique, led to the conclusion that the strongest pitch sensation in the BEP has the character of a weak fluctuating pure tones in noise, which corresponds to a frequency, equal or almost equal to the frequency of the phase transition. This result fits in with the predictions of the central spectrum theory, which, for instance, does not need the assumption of central lateral inhibition for explaining this pitch. Furthermore, it is shown that this theory can also predict the results obtained in lateralization measurements and BMLD measurements using BEP stimuli as well as related stimuli. The results are compared with the data obtained by Klein and Hartmann.  相似文献   

2.
In this paper previous experiments on auditory filter shapes in binaural masking experiments [A. Kohlrausch, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 84, 573-583 (1988)] are extended to a wider range of masker and signal durations. The masker was a dichotic broadband noise with frequency-dependent interaural parameters. The interaural phase difference of the masker was 0 below 500 Hz and pi above 500 Hz. Signal frequency varied between 200 and 800 Hz, and the signal was presented either monaurally (Sm) or binaurally in antiphase (S pi). In the first experiment, the masker duration was fixed at 500 ms and signals of 250 and 20 ms were used. In the second experiment, the signal duration was fixed at 20 ms, and the masker duration was reduced to 25 ms. The results from both experiments are consistent with studies using No or N pi maskers: The binaural masking level difference (BMLD) increases slightly for shorter test signals and decreases strongly for short maskers. The BMLD patterns of the first experiment are well described by the auditory-filter model derived for stationary test signals, if the additional influence of "off-frequency listening" for the short test signal is taken into account. The BMLDs resulting from the second experiment (25-ms masker), however, are much lower than predicted by this filter model This outcome supports previous observations that binaural unmasking becomes less effective for very short masker durations and indicates that this effect is even stronger for maskers with a complex structure of interaural parameters.  相似文献   

3.
Lateralization of complex binaural stimuli: a weighted-image model   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
This article describes a new model that predicts the subjective lateral position of bandpass stimuli. It is assumed, as in other models, that stimuli are bandpass filtered and rectified, and that the rectified outputs of filters with matching center frequencies undergo interaural cross correlation. The model specifies and utilizes the shape and location of assumed patterns of neural activity that describe the cross-correlation function. Individual modes of this function receive greater weighting if they are straighter (describing consistent interaural delay over frequency) and/or more central (describing interaural delays of smaller magnitude). This weighting of straightness and centrality is used by the model to predict the perceived laterality of several types of low-frequency bandpass stimuli with interaural time delays and/or phase shifts, including bandpass noise, amplitude-modulated stimuli with time-delayed envelopes, and bandpass-filtered clicks. This model is compared to other theories that describe lateralization in terms of the relative contributions of information in the envelopes and fine structures of binaural stimuli.  相似文献   

4.
A "simple" dichotic pitch arises when a single narrow band possesses a different interaural configuration from a surrounding broadband noise whose interaural configuration is uniform and correlated. Such pitches were created by interaurally decorrelating a narrow band (experiment 1) or by giving a narrow band a different interaural time difference from the noise (experiment 2). Using an adaptive forced-choice procedure, listeners adjusted the interaural intensity difference of "pointers" to match their lateralization to that of the dichotic pitches. The primary determinants of lateralization were the interaural configuration of the broadband noise (experiment 1), the center frequency of the narrow band (experiment 1), and its interaural configuration (experiment 2). The ability of two computational models to predict these results was evaluated. A version of the central-spectrum model [J. Raatgever and F. A. Bilsen, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 80, 429-441 (1986)] incorporating realistic frequency selectivity accounted for the main results of experiment 1 but not experiment 2. A new "reconstruction-comparison" model accounted for the main results of both experiments. To accommodate the variables shown to influence lateralization, this model segregates evidence of the dichotic pitch from the noise, reconstructs the cross-correlogram of the noise, and compares it with the cross-correlogram of the original stimulus.  相似文献   

5.
Running interaural cross correlation is a basic assumption to model the performance of the binaural auditory system. Although this concept is particularly suited to simulate psychoacoustic localization phenomena, there exist some localization effects which cannot be explained by pure cross correlation. In this paper a model of interaural cross correlation is extended by a "contralateral-inhibition mechanism" and by "monaural detectors" in order to simulate a wide range of psychoacoustic lateralization data. The extended model explains lateralization of pure tones with interaural time differences as well as with interaural level differences. Multiple images are predicted for tones with characteristic combinations of interaural signal parameters and for noise signals with different degrees of interaural cross correlation. The model is also capable of simulating dynamic lateralization phenomena, such as the "law of the first wave front" which is dealt with in a companion paper [Lindemann, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 80, 1623-1630 (1986)]. The present paper is restricted to a comparison of the model predictions for stationary signals with the results of dichotic listening experiments.  相似文献   

6.
Dichotic pitch perception reflects the auditory system's use of binaural cues to perceptually separate different sound sources and to determine the spatial location of sounds. Several studies were conducted to identify factors that influence children's dichotic pitch perception thresholds. An initial study of school children revealed an age-related improvement in thresholds for lateralizing dichotic pitch tones. In subsequent studies potential sensory and nonsensory limitations on young children's performance of dichotic pitch lateralization tasks were examined. A training study showed that with sufficient practice, young children lateralize dichotic pitch stimuli as well as adults, indicating an age difference in perceptual learning of the lateralization task. Changing the task requirements so that young children made a judgment about the pitch of dichotic pitch tones, rather than the spatial location of the tones, also resulted in significantly better thresholds. These findings indicate that nonsensory factors limit young children's performance of dichotic pitch tasks.  相似文献   

7.
The binaural interaction component (BIC=sum of monaural-true binaural) of the auditory brainstem response appears to reflect central binaural fusion/lateralization processes. Auditory middle-latency responses (AMLRs) are more robust and may reflect more completely such binaural processing. The AMLR also demonstrates such binaural interaction. The fusion of dichotically presented tones with an interaural frequency difference (IFD) offers another test of the extent to which electrophysiological and psychoacoustical measures agree. The effect of IFDs on both the BIC of the AMLR and a psychoacoustical measure of binaural fusion thus were examined. The perception of 20-ms tone bursts at/near 500 Hz with increasing IFDs showed, first, a deviated sound image from the center of the head, followed by clearly separate pitch percepts in each ear. Thresholds of detection of sound deviation and separation (i.e., nonfusion) were found to be 57 and 209 Hz, respectively. However, magnitudes of BICs of the AMLR were found to remain nearly. constant for IFDs up to the 400-Hz (limit of range tested), suggesting that the AMLR-BIC does not provide an objective index of this aspect of binaural processing, at least not under the conditions examined. The nature of lateralization due to IFDs and the concept of critical bands for binaural fusion are also discussed. Further research appears warranted to investigate the significance of the lack of effect of IFDs on the AMLR-BIC. Finally, the IFD paradigm itself would seem useful in that it permits determination of the limit for nonfusion of sounds presented binaurally, a limit not accessible via more conventional paradigms involving interaural time, phase, or intensity differences.  相似文献   

8.
The binaural coherence edge pitch (BICEP) is a dichotic broadband noise pitch effect similar to the binaural edge pitch (BEP). The BICEP stimulus is made by summing spectrally dense sine wave components with random phases. The interaural phase angle is a constant (0 or pi) for components with frequencies below (or above) a chosen edge frequency, and it is a random variable for the remaining components. The chosen edge frequency is a coherence edge because the noises to the two ears are mutually coherent within any band of frequencies on one side of the edge and they are mutually incoherent in any band on the other side. Pitch-matching experiments show that the BICEP exists for coherence edge frequencies between about 300 and 1000 Hz. It is matched by a pure-tone frequency that differs from the edge frequency by 5% to 10%. The matching frequency lies on the incoherent side of the edge, an important result that is consistent with the way that the equalization-cancellation model has been applied to binaural pitch effects, especially the BEP. The results of BICEP experiments depend upon whether the coherent components are presented in 0 or pi interaural phase for some listeners but not for all. The BICEP persists if the noise to one of the ears is delayed, but it becomes weaker and less well matched as the delay increases beyond 2 ms. The BICEP does not depend on whether the component amplitudes are all created equal or are given a Rayleigh distribution. Some reliable pitch sensation exists even when the component amplitudes are entirely independent in the two ears, so long as the phase coherence conditions of the BICEP stimulus are maintained. The existence of the BICEP is a challenge for current models of dichotic pitch because none of them predicts all its features.  相似文献   

9.
The shape of the auditory filter was calculated from binaural masking experiments. Two different types of maskers were used in the study, a masker that was interaurally in phase at all frequencies (No), and a masker with an interaural phase difference of 0 below 500 Hz and of pi above 500 Hz. The test-signal frequency varied between 200 and 800 Hz, and the test signal was presented either monaurally (Sm) or binaurally in antiphase (S pi). By comparing the masked thresholds from the two experimental conditions, the following conclusion can be drawn: The threshold of the test signal is only affected by the masker phase within a narrow frequency range around the test frequency. Thus, for test-signal frequencies well above or below 500 Hz, no influence of the phase transition on the BMLD is observed, and normal masked thresholds for No and N pi maskers are obtained. For test frequencies around 500 Hz, the step in interaural phase difference leads to a decrease in the interaural correlation of the masker within the critical band around the test-signal frequency. This results in strong threshold changes for both monaural and binaural signals. A calculation of the auditory filter shape from the masked threshold values was performed under the assumption that the masked threshold is only dependent on the interaural cross correlation of the masker within the filter band. Using the formula of the EC theory for the relation between masker correlation and BMLD, the experimental data are well described by a trapezoidal filter with an equivalent rectangular bandwidth of 80 to 84 Hz.  相似文献   

10.
The cortical mechanisms of perceptual segregation of concurrent sound sources were examined, based on binaural detection of interaural timing differences. Auditory event-related potentials were measured from 11 healthy subjects. Binaural stimuli were created by introducing a dichotic delay of 500-ms duration to a narrow frequency region within a broadband noise, and resulted in a perception of a centrally located noise and a right-lateralized pitch (dichotic pitch). In separate listening conditions, subjects actively discriminated and responded to randomly interleaved binaural and control stimuli, or ignored random stimuli while watching silent cartoons. In a third listening condition subjects ignored stimuli presented in homogenous blocks. For all listening conditions, the dichotic pitch stimulus elicited an object-related negativity (ORN) at a latency of about 150-250 ms after stimulus onset. When subjects were required to actively respond to stimuli, the ORN was followed by a P400 wave with a latency of about 320-420 ms. These results support and extend a two-stage model of auditory scene analysis in which acoustic streams are automatically parsed into component sound sources based on source-relevant cues, followed by a controlled process involving identification and generation of a behavioral response.  相似文献   

11.
The changes in perception and in the binaural difference waveform (BD) for dichotic clicks with interaural time and level differences (ITDs and ILDs) are compared. Only beta, the first major peak of the BD, correlated with the perceptual measurements. Whenever beta is clearly present, the clicks are perceived as a unitary fused image. Whenever the clicks are perceived as not fused, beta is undetectable by our methods. The amplitude of beta remains nearly constant as the ITD is increased to about 1 ms, while the click's position is perceived as moving from midline toward the leading ear. Over about the next 0.2 ms, beta becomes undetectable, as the image stops moving and loses its fused quality. As the ILD is increased, beta amplitude decreases gradually, while the image remains unitary and moves toward the unattenuated earphone. Thus beta becomes undetectable for ILDs of 30 to 35 dB, and the dichotic clicks become indistinguishable from monotic clicks for ILDs of 44 to 53 dB. The ITD and ILD matching curve for beta latency is similar to the ITD and ILD psychophysical matching curve for lateralization. These results suggest that beta is a physiological correlate of the categorical percept, binaural fusion, and is generated by a brainstem structure essential for the preception of click lateralization.  相似文献   

12.
Two experiments examined the relationship between temporal pitch (and, more generally, rate) perception and auditory lateralization. Both used dichotic pulse trains that were filtered into the same high (3,900-5,400-Hz) frequency region in order to eliminate place-of-excitation cues. In experiment 1, a 1-s periodic pulse train of rate Fr was presented to one ear, and a pulse train of rate 2Fr was presented to the other. In the "synchronous" condition, every other pulse in the 2Fr train was simultaneous with a pulse in the opposite ear. In each trial, subjects concentrated on one of the two binaural images produced by this mixture: they matched its perceived location by adjusting the interaural level difference (ILD) of a bandpass noise, and its rate/pitch was then matched by adjusting the rate of a regular pulse train. The results showed that at low Fr (e.g., 2 Hz), subjects heard two pulse trains of rate Fr, one in the "higher rate" ear, and one in the middle of the head. At higher Fr (>25 Hz) subjects heard two pulse trains on opposite sides of the midline, with the image on the higher rate side having a higher pitch than that on the "lower rate" side. The results were compared to those in a control condition, in which the pulses in the two ears were asynchronous. This comparison revealed a duplex region at Fr > 25 Hz, where across-ear synchrony still affected the perceived locations of the pulse trains, but did not affect their pitches. Experiment 2 used a 1.4-s 200-Hz dichotic pulse train, whose first 0.7 s contained a constant interaural time difference (ITD), after which the sign of the ITD alternated between subsequent pulses. Subjects matched the location and then the pitch of the "new" sound that started halfway through the pulse train. The matched location became more lateralized with increasing ITD, but subjects always matched a pitch near 200 Hz, even though the rate of pulses sharing the new ITD was only 100 Hz. It is concluded from both experiments that temporal pitch perception is not driven by the output of binaural mechanisms.  相似文献   

13.
Subjects judged the loudness and the lateral position of dichotic transient signals, which were presented at equal and unequal levels, synchronously and asynchronously, to the two ears. Binaural loudness summation of clicks does not obey a law of linear addition: It is partial at low level and superadditive at high level. Supersummation is greater for interaurally delayed clicks than for coincidental ones. The relation between click loudness and sound pressure (over moderate SLs) can be described as a power function with a greater exponent for the binaural function. Lateral positions spread over a greater range for interaural level differences than for interaural time differences. The time-intensity trading ratio was greater than is typically reported for tones. When sound lateralization was induced by interaural time difference, but not by intensity difference, a virtually perfect negative correlation between loudness and extent of off-center displacement existed.  相似文献   

14.
The ability to segregate two spectrally and temporally overlapping signals based on differences in temporal envelope structure and binaural cues was investigated. Signals were a harmonic tone complex (HTC) with 20 Hz fundamental frequency and a bandpass noise (BPN). Both signals had interaural differences of the same absolute value, but with opposite signs to establish lateralization to different sides of the medial plane, such that their combination yielded two different spatial configurations. As an indication for segregation ability, threshold interaural time and level differences were measured for discrimination between these spatial configurations. Discrimination based on interaural level differences was good, although absolute thresholds depended on signal bandwidth and center frequency. Discrimination based on interaural time differences required the signals' temporal envelope structures to be sufficiently different. Long-term interaural cross-correlation patterns or long-term averaged patterns after equalization-cancellation of the combined signals did not provide information for the discrimination. The binaural system must, therefore, have been capable of processing changes in interaural time differences within the period of the harmonic tone complex, suggesting that monaural information from the temporal envelopes influences the use of binaural information in the perceptual organization of signal components.  相似文献   

15.
Recent studies of multiple sclerosis (MS) and stroke patients suggested a correlation between two patterns of abnormal performance in lateralization tasks and two sites of pontine lesions. Most patients who had lesions below or at the superior olivary complex (SOC) perceived all interaural differences in binaural stimuli as small, while most patients who had lesions above the SOC perceived all interaural differences as large. The two abnormal performance patterns occurred for interaural time differences (ITD) and/or for interaural level differences (ILD). The present model proposes a multi-level hierarchical brainstem structure that estimates ITD and ILD. The first level seeks dissimilarity between the left and right inputs and a second level looks for similarity between the two sides' inputs. Each level is modeled as an ensemble of neural arrays in which each unit performs a logic or arithmetic function. The inputs are simulations of auditory nerve responses to broadband stimuli. Simulations yield good correspondence to the effect of both locations of pontine lesions on binaural performance.  相似文献   

16.
Either an interaural phase shift or level difference was introduced to a narrow section of broadband noise in order to measure the acuity of the binaural system to segregate a narrowband from a broadband stimulus. Listeners were asked to indicate whether this dichotic noise or a totally diotic noise was presented in a single-interval procedure. Thresholds for interaural phase and level differences were estimated from four point psychometric functions. These thresholds were determined for three bandwidths of interaurally altered noise (2, 10, and 100 Hz) centered at four center frequencies (200, 500, 1000, and 1600 Hz). Thresholds were lowest when the interaurally altered band of noise was centered at 500 Hz, and thresholds increased as the bandwidth of the interaurally altered noise decreased. Performance did not exceed 75% correct when either an interaural phase shift (180 degrees) or interaural level difference (50 dB) was introduced to a 100 Hz band of noise centered at frequencies higher than 1600 Hz. In a second set of conditions, performance was measured when both an interaural phase shift and level difference were presented in a 10-Hz-wide band of noise centered at 500 Hz. A version of the Durlach E-C model was able to account for a great deal of the data. The results are discussed in terms of the Huggins dichotic pitch.  相似文献   

17.
A dichotic pitch known as Fourcin Pitch (FP) is perceived when two independent interaurally delayed white noises are presented simultaneously. Experiments in the past mainly dealt with stimulus situations where one interaural delay was smaller than 4 ms and the other was in the range 0-10 ms. Here, experiments are reported showing a novel pitch percept that occurs when one noise is interaurally delayed at the left ear and the other noise is about equally delayed at the right ear, for delays in the range of 4-10 ms. It is shown that, for both the previously reported FP and the novel pitch, the Central Spectrum (CS) theory [Raatgever and Bilsen, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 80, 429-441 (1986)] provides a correct prediction of both the pitch value and the lateral position of the pitch image, where other current theories partly or completely fail. Further experiments indicate that the lateralized position of the pitch image is essentially not influenced by an interaural intensity difference (IID). This is also predicted by the CS model and leads to the generalized conclusion that interaural time and intensity differences are processed separately in the auditory system until, at least, the level where the dichotic pitch is generated.  相似文献   

18.
In assigning binaural ongoing time differences (phase) as the cue for localization of low frequencies, and binaural intensity differences as the cue for localization of high frequencies, the duplex theory has successfully accounted for human directional hearing of tones. Sensitivity of monkeys to these cues was examined in two experiments. The dependencies on frequency of interaural intensity difference thresholds (lateralization experiment I) and time difference thresholds (lateralization experiment II) were determined behaviorally on three monkeys (M. nemestrina). The range of frequencies was from 125 Hz to 8 kHz in experiment I and from 250 Hz to 2 kHz in experiment II. The results indicate that the duplex theory is applicable to monkeys. However, monkeys are less sensitive than man to both binaural cues. The shortest time disparity monkeys discriminate is 42 microseconds at 1.5 kHz and the smallest intensity difference is 3.5 dB at 500 Hz. Good agreement between the present findings and localization measurements [C. H. Brown et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 63, 1484-1492 (1978)] suggests: (a) that monkeys utilize time disparity cues through higher frequencies than man; and (b) that inaccurate localization by monkeys at high frequencies reflects decreasing sensitivity to interaural intensity cues.  相似文献   

19.
In a test sound consisting of a burst of pink noise, an arbitrarily selected target frequency band can be "enhanced" by the previous presentation of a similar noise with a spectral notch in the target frequency region. As a result of the enhancement, the test sound evokes a pitch sensation corresponding to the pitch of the target band. Here, a pitch comparison task was used to assess enhancement. In the first experiment, a stronger enhancement effect was found when the test sound and its precursor had the same interaural time difference (ITD) than when they had opposite ITDs. Two subsequent experiments were concerned with the audibility of an instance of dichotic pitch in binaural test sounds preceded by precursors. They showed that it is possible to enhance a frequency region on the sole basis of ITD manipulations, using spectrally identical test sounds and precursors. However, the observed effects were small. A major goal of this study was to test the hypothesis that enhancement originates at least in part from neural adaptation processes taking place at a central level of the auditory system. The data failed to provide strong support for this hypothesis.  相似文献   

20.
A commonly accepted physiological model for lateralization of low-frequency sounds by interaural time delay (ITD) stipulates that binaural comparison neurons receive input from frequency-matched channels from each ear. Here, the effects of hypothetical interaural frequency mismatches on this model are reported. For this study, the cat's auditory system peripheral to the binaural comparison neurons was represented by a neurophysiologically derived model, and binaural comparison neurons were represented by cross-correlators. The results of the study indicate that, for binaural comparison neurons receiving input from one cochlear channel from each ear, interaural CF mismatches may serve to either augment or diminish the effective difference in ipsilateral and contralateral axonal time delays from the periphery to the binaural comparison neuron. The magnitude of this increase or decrease in the effective time delay difference can be up to 400 microseconds for CF mismatches of 0.2 octaves or less for binaural neurons with CFs between 250 Hz and 2.5 kHz. For binaural comparison neurons with nominal CFs near 500 Hz, the 25-microsecond effective time delay difference caused by a 0.012-octave CF mismatch is equal to the ITD previously shown to be behaviorally sufficient for the cat to lateralize a low-frequency sound source.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号