共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
M G Heinz H S Colburn L H Carney 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》2001,110(4):2065-2084
The perceptual significance of the cochlear amplifier was evaluated by predicting level-discrimination performance based on stochastic auditory-nerve (AN) activity. Performance was calculated for three models of processing: the optimal all-information processor (based on discharge times), the optimal rate-place processor (based on discharge counts), and a monaural coincidence-based processor that uses a non-optimal combination of rate and temporal information. An analytical AN model included compressive magnitude and level-dependent-phase responses associated with the cochlear amplifier, and high-, medium-, and low-spontaneous-rate (SR) fibers with characteristic frequencies (CFs) spanning the AN population. The relative contributions of nonlinear magnitude and nonlinear phase responses to level encoding were compared by using four versions of the model, which included and excluded the nonlinear gain and phase responses in all possible combinations. Nonlinear basilar-membrane (BM) phase responses are robustly encoded in near-CF AN fibers at low frequencies. Strongly compressive BM responses at high frequencies near CF interact with the high thresholds of low-SR AN fibers to produce large dynamic ranges. Coincidence performance based on a narrow range of AN CFs was robust across a wide dynamic range at both low and high frequencies, and matched human performance levels. Coincidence performance based on all CFs demonstrated the "near-miss" to Weber's law at low frequencies and the high-frequency "mid-level bump." Monaural coincidence detection is a physiologically realistic mechanism that is extremely general in that it can utilize AN information (average-rate, synchrony, and nonlinear-phase cues) from all SR groups. 相似文献
2.
The effect of the filter bank on fundamental frequency (F0) discrimination was examined in four Nucleus CI24 cochlear implant subjects for synthetic stylized vowel-like stimuli. The four tested filter banks differed in cutoff frequencies, amount of overlap between filters, and shape of the filters. To assess the effects of temporal pitch cues on F0 discrimination, temporal fluctuations were removed above 10 Hz in one condition and above 200 Hz in another. Results indicate that F0 discrimination based upon place pitch cues is possible, but just-noticeable differences exceed 1 octave or more depending on the filter bank used. Increasing the frequency resolution in the F0 range improves the F0 discrimination based upon place pitch cues. The results of F0 discrimination based upon place pitch agree with a model that compares the centroids of the electrical excitation pattern. The addition of temporal fluctuations up to 200 Hz significantly improves F0 discrimination. Just-noticeable differences using both place and temporal pitch cues range from 6% to 60%. Filter banks that do not resolve the higher harmonics provided the best temporal pitch cues, because temporal pitch cues are clearest when the fluctuation on all channels is at F0 and preferably in phase. 相似文献
3.
Frequency modulation detection limens (FMDLs) were measured for five hearing-impaired (HI) subjects for carrier frequencies f(c) = 1000, 4000, and 6000 Hz, using modulation frequencies f(m) = 2 and 10 Hz and levels of 20 dB sensation level and 90 dB SPL. FMDLs were smaller for f(m) = 10 than for f(m) = 2 Hz for the two higher f(c), but not for f(c) = 1000 Hz. FMDLs were also determined with additional random amplitude modulation (AM), to disrupt excitation-pattern cues. The disruptive effect was larger for f(m) = 10 than for f(m) = 2 Hz. The smallest disruption occurred for f(m) = 2 Hz and f(c) = 1000 Hz. AM detection thresholds for normal-hearing and HI subjects were measured for the same f(c) and f(m) values. Performance was better for the HI subjects for both f(m). AM detection was much better for f(m) = 10 than for f(m) = 2 Hz. Additional tests showed that most HI subjects could discriminate temporal fine structure (TFS) at 800 Hz. The results are consistent with the idea that, for f(m) = 2 Hz and f(c) = 1000 Hz, frequency modulation (FM) detection was partly based on the use of TFS information. For higher carrier frequencies and for all carrier frequencies with f(m) = 10 Hz, FM detection was probably based on place cues. 相似文献
4.
J M Kates 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1985,77(4):1529-1534
In this paper we describe a monaural auditory signal-processing model for the perception of coloration. The model gives a central spectrum display of a stationary input signal. The central spectrum level for a nerve fiber tuned to a given frequency is computed as a combination of the average firing rate and the firing synchronized to the center frequency of the nerve. The model incorporates a critical-band filter bank, steady-state representations of the average and synchronized firing rates, and temporal integration. The central spectrum model, when used to process simulated data, accurately predicts the perception of coloration in filtered Gaussian noise. 相似文献
5.
6.
This study focuses on the extraction of robust acoustic cues of labial and alveolar voiceless obstruents in German and their acoustic differences in the speech signal to distinguish them in place and manner of articulation. The investigated obstruents include the affricates [pf] and [ts], the fricatives [f] and [s] and the stops [p] and [t]. The target sounds were analyzed in word-initial and word-medial positions. The speech data for the analysis were recorded in a natural environment, deliberately containing background noise to extract robust cues only. Three methods of acoustic analysis were chosen: (1) temporal measurements to distinguish the respective obstruents in manner of articulation, (2) static spectral characteristics in terms of logarithmic distance measure to distinguish place of articulation, and (3) amplitudinal analysis of discrete frequency bands as a dynamic approach to place distinction. The results reveal that the duration of the target phonemes distinguishes these in manner of articulation. Logarithmic distance measure, as well as relative amplitude analysis of discrete frequency bands, identifies place of articulation. The present results contribute to the question, which properties are robust with respect to variation in the speech signal. 相似文献
7.
8.
9.
《Physics letters. A》1987,119(7):337-339
A leaky cavity is an example of a dissipative quantum system. The thermal spectrum of such a system can be expressed exactly in terms of a generalized density of states πc(ω) for the cavity. The latter is evaluated for a string model; compared to the zero leakage limit, each δ-function is replaced by a pair of poles in the ω-plane, related to the poles of the S-matrix. 相似文献
10.
11.
Subjects discriminated a "standard" pair of tone bursts (T1, T2) from a "comparison" pair (T1 + delta t, T2 + delta f), containing increments in the duration delta t of the first burst and/or the frequency delta f of the second burst. The threshold (d' = 2.0) for delta t was measured as a function of delta f, and the threshold for delta f as a function of delta t. The integration of increments in duration and frequency was studied as a function of the spectral and temporal separation between T1 and T2. A trade-off between the values of delta t and delta f required for d' = 2.0 performance was observed. This integration takes place when delta t, delta f occur simultaneously in the same spectral region, and when they occur separated by up to 120 ms, or by up to a full octave. The efficiency of integration was similar for all conditions of temporal and spectral separation studied, because the discriminability of delta t and of delta f is also nearly uniform across experimental conditions. The results from all experimental conditions are adequately described by a vector summation model derived from TSD. In a subsidiary experiment, subjects categorized pure tones varying in duration and frequency as "high" or "low" in pitch and "long" or "short" in duration. It was found that combined variations in duration and frequency result in essentially independent perceptual processes, although pitch has a small effect upon the perceived duration. It is concluded that spectral-temporal integration is a general ability operating in a variety of stimulus conditions.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS) 相似文献
12.
A. V. Smol’yakov 《Acoustical Physics》2006,52(3):331-337
A model is developed for the statistical characteristics of the turbulent pressure field in a boundary layer. It is shown that the coherence scales are limited at low frequencies by the finiteness of the boundary layer thickness and at high frequencies by the effect of viscous forces acting in the flow. A relationship between the behavior of the coherence scales and that of the power spectra is demonstrated. The model is characterized by convertibility, which allows changes from cross spectra to wavenumber-frequency spectra and back via Fourier transformations. Calculations performed using the proposed model agree well with experimental results. 相似文献
13.
Experiments and model calculations were performed to study the influence of within-channel cues versus across-channel cues in comodulation masking release (CMR). A class of CMR experiments is considered that are characterized by a single (unmodulated or modulated) bandpass noise masker with variable bandwidth centered at the signal frequency. A modulation-filterbank model suggested by Dau et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102, 2892-2905 (1997)] was employed to quantitatively predict the experimental data. Effects of varying masker bandwidth, center frequency, modulator bandwidth, modulator type, and signal duration on CMR were examined. In addition, the effect of band limiting the noise before or after modulation was shown to influence the CMR in the same way as a systematic variation of the modulation depth. It is demonstrated that a single-channel analysis, which analyzes only the information from one peripheral channel, quantitatively accounts for the CMR in most cases, indicating that an across-channel process is generally not necessary for simulating results from this class of CMR experiments. True across-channel processes may be found in another class of CMR experiments. 相似文献
14.
The evolution of the experimental frequency width of symmetric modes of an aluminum plate is studied as a function of the angle of incidence below the first critical angle. It is found that the frequency width predicted by resonant scattering theory, corrected for the directivity of emitter and receiver, generally explains the experimental frequency width well. However, large discrepancies remain for the frequency width of the S1 mode at angles of incidence larger than 9 degrees. It is demonstrated that these are caused by not taking into account the complex nature of the slowness of the plate mode. This suggests that there is a need for a theory that models the interaction of a beam of ultrasound, bounded in space and time, with an elastic plate. 相似文献
15.
Perception of static and dynamic acoustic cues to place of articulation in initial stop consonants 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
D Kewley-Port D B Pisoni M Studdert-Kennedy 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1983,73(5):1779-1793
Two recent accounts of the acoustic cues which specify place of articulation in syllable-initial stop consonants claim that they are located in the initial portions of the CV waveform and are context-free. Stevens and Blumstein [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 64, 1358-1368 (1978)] have described the perceptually relevant spectral properties of these cues as static, while Kewley-Port [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 73, 322-335 (1983)] describes these cues as dynamic. Three perceptual experiments were conducted to test predictions derived from these accounts. Experiment 1 confirmed that acoustic cues for place of articulation are located in the initial 20-40 ms of natural stop-vowel syllables. Next, short synthetic CV's modeled after natural syllables were generated using either a digital, parallel-resonance synthesizer in experiment 2 or linear prediction synthesis in experiment 3. One set of synthetic stimuli preserved the static spectral properties proposed by Stevens and Blumstein. Another set of synthetic stimuli preserved the dynamic properties suggested by Kewley-Port. Listeners in both experiments identified place of articulation significantly better from stimuli which preserved dynamic acoustic properties than from those based on static onset spectra. Evidently, the dynamic structure of the initial stop-vowel articulatory gesture can be preserved in context-free acoustic cues which listeners use to identify place of articulation. 相似文献
16.
R L Smith M L Brachman R D Frisina 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1985,78(4):1310-1316
Adaptation of auditory-nerve responses was investigated by applying increments and decrements in intensity to an ongoing tonal background. The change in firing rate produced by a change in intensity was obtained as a function of the time delay from the onset of the background to the onset of the change in intensity. The initial change in firing rate was measured using both small (1 ms) and large (10 ms) time intervals in order to evaluate properties of rapid and short-term adaptation, respectively. Consistent with previous results, the incremental and decremental responses measured with large windows were independent of time delay and the amount of prior adaptation. A similar additivity was observed for the incremental response measured with a small time window. In contrast, the decremental response measured with a small window decreased with increasing time delay and in proportion to the decrease in firing rate produced by the background. A similar decrease was observed in the response modulation produced by sinusoidal amplitude modulation. It was concluded that sensitivity to decrements in intensity decreases during adaptation, so that this response component does not reflect the additivity inherent in other aspects of adaptation. 相似文献
17.
Mackie M 《Physical review letters》2003,91(17):173004
We theoretically investigate the effect of anomalous quantum correlations on the light-induced frequency shift in the photoassociation spectrum of a Bose-Einstein condensate. Anomalous quantum correlations arise because, although formed from a pair of zero-momentum condensate atoms, a condensate molecule need not dissociate back to the atomic condensate, but may just as well form a noncondensate atom pair with equal and opposite momentum, i.e., due to rogue photodissociation. The uncorrelated frequency shift of the photoassociation spectrum is to the red and linearly dependent on the laser intensity I. In contrast, anomalous correlations due to rogue dissociation lead to a blueshifted photoassociation spectrum. For sufficiently low light intensities, the rogue blueshift is dominant and proportional to sqrt[I]. 相似文献
18.
19.
Longhi S 《Optics letters》2005,30(7):786-788
It is shown that a frequency mode-locked laser with a sinusoidal sweep of modulation frequency around a mode-locking condition represents an ideal optical system for observing in the spectral domain the phenomena of dynamic localization and Bloch oscillations of electrons in an ideal solid placed in an external ac electric field. 相似文献
20.
S Gatehouse 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1992,92(3):1258-1268
At high presentation levels, normally aided ears yield better performance for speech identification than normally unaided ears, while at low presentation levels the converse is true [S. Gatehouse, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 86, 2103-2106 (1989)]. To explain this process further, the speech identification abilities of four subjects with bilateral symmetric sensorineural hearing impairment were investigated following provision of a single hearing aid. Results showed significant increases in the benefit from amplifying speech in the aided ear, but not in the control ear. In addition, a headphone simulation of the unaided condition for the fitted ear shows a decrease in speech identification. The benefits from providing a particular frequency spectrum do not emerge immediately, but over a time course of at least 6-12 weeks. The findings support the existence of perceptual acclimatization effects, and call into question short-term methods of hearing aid evaluation and selection by comparative speech identification tests. 相似文献