共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Pure-tone intensity discrimination and energy detection 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
2.
Kajikawa S Fais L Mugitani R Werker JF Amano S 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》2006,120(4):2278-2284
This study explored sensitivity to word-level phonotactic patterns in English and Japanese monolingual infants. Infants at the ages of 6, 12, and 18 months were tested on their ability to discriminate between test words using a habituation-switch experimental paradigm. All of the test words, neek, neeks, and neekusu, are phonotactically legitimate for English, whereas the first two words are critically noncanonical in Japanese. The language-specific phonotactical congruence influenced infants' performance in discrimination. English-learning infants could discriminate between neek and neeks at the age of 18 months, but Japanese infants could not. There was a similar developmental pattern for infants of both language groups for discrimination of neek and neeks, but Japanese infants showed a different trajectory from English infants for neekusu/neeks. These differences reflect the different status of these word patterns with respect to the phonotactics of both languages, and reveal early sensitivity to subtle phonotactic and language input patterns in each language. 相似文献
3.
The present study investigated the effect of increasing the number and frequency range of background components upon the detection of an increment to the intensity of the center component. Previous studies [e.g., Green et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 75, 1163-1167 (1984)] have demonstrated that, for background stimuli consisting of logarithmically spaced pure-tone components, as the number of background components is increased, the threshold for detection of an increment to the center component is decreased. The present study investigated this effect using harmonically spaced, as well as logarithmically spaced, backgrounds, because many natural sounds, including voiced speech sounds, consist of harmonically spaced components. Two conditions were employed for both types of backgrounds, fixed-level and roving-level within a trial. Group results revealed a small decrease in threshold with increasing number of components only for the fixed-level, logarithmically spaced backgrounds. No decrease in threshold was observed for the other three conditions, including the roving-level, logarithmically spaced backgrounds which were the same as those used by Green et al. (1984). The present results suggest that the decrease in signal threshold with increasing number of background components exists only under limited conditions and that it is a highly individual phenomenon. The present results also suggest that the effect does not occur for harmonically spaced complexes such as most natural sounds including voiced speech. 相似文献
4.
C Mitchell R Gillette J Vernon P Herman 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1970,48(2):531-535
5.
6.
Shi R Cutler A Werker J Cruickshank M 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》2006,119(6):EL61-EL67
High-frequency functors are arguably among the earliest perceived word forms and may assist extraction of initial vocabulary items. Canadian 11- and 8-month-olds were familiarized to pseudo-nouns following either a high-frequency functor the or a low-frequency functor her versus phonetically similar mispronunciations of each, kuh and ler, and then tested for recognition of the pseudo-nouns. A preceding the (but not kuh, her, ler) facilitated extraction of the pseudo-nouns for 11-month-olds; the is thus well-specified in form for these infants. However, both the and kuh (but not her-ler) facilitated segmentation for 8-month-olds, suggesting an initial underspecified representation of high-frequency functors. 相似文献
7.
Mugitani R Fais L Kajikawa S Werker JF Amano S 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》2007,122(3):1332
Japanese infants at the ages of 6, 12, and 18 months were tested on their ability to discriminate three nonsense words with different phonotactic status: canonical keetsu, noncanonical but possible keets, and noncanonical and impossible keet. The results showed that 12 and 18 months olds discriminate the keets/keetsu pair, but infants in all age groups fail to discriminate the keets/keet pair. Taken together with the findings in our previous study [Kajikawa et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 120(4), 2278-2284 (2006)], these results suggest that Japanese infants develop the perceptual sensitivity for native phonotactics after 6 months of age, and that this sensitivity is limited to canonical patterns at this early developmental stage. 相似文献
8.
M D Beecher 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1974,55(1):196-198
9.
R N Aslin 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1989,86(2):582-590
Difference limens (DLs) for linear frequency transitions using a 1.0-kHz pulsed-tone standard were obtained from 6- to 9-month-old human infants in a series of three experiments. A repeating standard "yes-no" operant headturning technique and an adaptive staircase (tracking) procedure were used to obtain difference limens from a total of 71 infants. The DLs for 300-ms upward and downward linear frequency sweeps were approximately 3%-4% when the repeating standard was an unmodulated 1.0-kHz pulsed tone of 300-ms duration. These DLs for frequency sweeps were not significantly different from DLs for frequency increments and decrements using 330-ms pulsed tones [J. M. Sinnott and R. N. Aslin, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 78, 1986-1992 (1985)]. The DLs for frequency sweeps of 50 ms appended to the beginning or the end of a 250-ms unmodulated 1.0-kHz tone were approximately 6%-7%. This greater DL for brief frequency sweeps was confirmed by varying the duration but not the extent of the sweep. Finally, DLs were greater than 50% when the repeating standard was a 50-ms rising or falling frequency sweep appended to the beginning of a 250-ms unmodulated 1.0-kHz tone. These results suggest that rapid frequency transitions are much more difficult to discriminate from frequency transitions of the same category (rising or falling) than from either a frequency transition of the opposite category (falling or rising) or an unmodulated tone.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS) 相似文献
10.
《光学技术》2013,(3):228-232
研究了人眼在不同观察时间下的亮度对比度敏感函数。从CIE规定的颜色中心中分别选取中性灰和橙色作为图片背景,并得到不同空间频率的矩形条纹图片。根据心理物理学的极限法进行视觉评价,测得在两个背景三种典型适应观察时间下人眼的亮度对比度敏感函数。比较了中性灰和橙色背景下人眼亮度对比度敏感函数数据,并拟合得到在不同背景及对比度适应时间下的亮度对比度敏感特性曲线。结果表明,当观察时间相同时,橙色背景的亮度对比敏感函数值比中性灰背景的亮度对比度敏感函数值小;随着人眼对条纹观察时间的延长,人眼亮度对比度敏感函数的峰值及其对应的空间频率也随之增大,与背景颜色无关。 相似文献
11.
Pitch-intensity functions and psychophysical tuning curves (PTC's) were measured in ten listeners with sensorineural impairments of presumed cochlear origin. Masking patterns, frequency jnd's, diplacusis measurements, and octave adjustments were also obtained for selected conditions in selected listeners. The results showed a tendency for increased frequency jnd's and increased pitch-matching variability in frequency regions where frequency resolution, as determined by PTC Q10 estimates, was degraded. The results also showed exaggerated pitch-level effects, both in regions where frequency resolution was degraded and, in many cases, in regions where thresholds and frequency resolution were apparently normal. The usual manifestation of exaggerated pitch-level effect was an abnormally large negative pitch shift with increasing level, particularly at low frequencies. The limited data from diplacusis measurements and octave adjustments suggest that the exaggerated negative pitch shifts are the consequence of a large increase in pitch at low stimulus levels which "recruits" at higher levels. These results are difficult to explain with simple tonotopic models, or presently formulated temporal models, of pure-tone pitch encoding. 相似文献
12.
This study examined the ability of cochlear implant users and normal-hearing subjects to perform auditory stream segregation of pure tones. An adaptive, rhythmic discrimination task was used to assess stream segregation as a function of frequency separation of the tones. The results for normal-hearing subjects were consistent with previously published observations (L.P.A.S van Noorden, Ph.D. dissertation, Eindhoven University of Technology, Eindhoven, The Netherlands 1975), suggesting that auditory stream segregation increases with increasing frequency separation. For cochlear implant users, there appeared to be a range of pure-tone streaming abilities, with some subjects demonstrating streaming comparable to that of normal-hearing individuals, and others possessing much poorer streaming abilities. The variability in pure-tone streaming of cochlear implant users was correlated with speech perception in both steady-state noise and multi-talker babble. Moderate, statistically significant correlations between streaming and both measures of speech perception in noise were observed, with better stream segregation associated with better understanding of speech in noise. These results suggest that auditory stream segregation is a contributing factor in the ability to understand speech in background noise. The inability of some cochlear implant users to perform stream segregation may therefore contribute to their difficulties in noise backgrounds. 相似文献
13.
14.
Frequency and intensity DLs were measured in 26 human infants (ages 7-9 months) and six young adults using a repeating standard "yes-no" operant headturning technique and an adaptive staircase (tracking) psychophysical procedure. Subjects were visually reinforced for responding to frequency increments, frequency decrements, intensity increments, or intensity decrements in an ongoing train of 1.0-kHz tone bursts, and stimulus control was monitored using randomly interleaved probe and catch trials. Infants were easily conditioned to respond to both increments and decrements in frequency, and DLs ranged from 11-29 Hz, while adult DLs ranged from 3-5 Hz. Infants also easily discriminated intensity increments, and DLs ranged from 3-12 dB, while adult DLs ranged from 1-2 dB. No infants successfully discriminated intensity decrements, although adults experienced no difficulty with this task and produced DLs similar to those for increments. The apparent inability of infants to discriminate intensity decrements suggests that the infant CNS may not be well adapted to monitor rate decreases in populations of peripheral auditory neurons. 相似文献
15.
人眼对比度敏感视觉特性及模型研究 总被引:9,自引:1,他引:8
人眼视觉系统特性是图像显示、处理、理解等技术的重要依据,通过实验测试和理论分析研究人眼的视觉特性,使人眼视觉特性模型化,对促进光电成像技术的发展和应用具有重要的作用。结合前人拟合人眼对比度敏感函数和理论提取其数学模型的经验,对用CRT显示器显示目标光栅,在暗室环境下测量三种平均亮度的人眼对比度敏感数据采用线性调制作用下的指数形式的方法进行了拟合,并从理论上得出了一种新的CSF数学模型。通过与国外拟合结果的对比,表明此模型是一种更好的人眼对比对敏感视觉特性模型。 相似文献
16.
Pure-tone threshold estimation from extrapolated distortion product otoacoustic emission I/O-functions in normal and cochlear hearing loss ears 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
A new method for direct pure-tone threshold estimation from input/output functions of distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) in humans is presented. Previous methods use statistical models relating DPOAE level to hearing threshold including additional parameters e.g., age or slope of DPOAE I/O-function. Here we derive a DPOAE threshold from extrapolated DPOAE I/O-functions directly. Cubic 2 f1-f2 distortion products and pure-tone threshold at f2 were measured at 51 frequencies between f2=500 Hz and 8 kHz at up to ten primary tone levels between L2=65 and 20 dB SPL in 30 normally hearing and 119 sensorineural hearing loss ears. Using an optimized primary tone level setting (L1 = 0.4L2 + 39 dB) that accounts for the nonlinear interaction of the two primaries at the DPOAE generation site at f2, the pressure of the 2 f1-f2 distortion product pDP is a linear function of the primary tone level L2. Linear regression yields correlation coefficients higher than 0.8 in the majority of the DPOAE I/O-functions. The linear behavior is sufficiently fulfilled for all frequencies in normal and impaired hearing. This suggests that the observed linear functional dependency is quite general. Extrapolating towards pDP=0 yields the DPOAE threshold for L2. There is a significant correlation between DPOAE threshold and pure-tone threshold (r=0.65, p<0.001). Thus, the DPOAEs that reflect the functioning of an essential element of peripheral sound processing enable a reliable estimation of cochlear hearing threshold up to hearing losses of 50 dBHL without any statistical data. 相似文献
17.
Differences in human and monkey sensitivity to acoustic cues underlying voicing contrasts 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Humans and monkeys were compared in their differential sensitivity to various acoustic cues underlying voicing contrasts specified by voice-onset time (VOT) in utterance-initial stop consonants. A low-uncertainty repeating standard AX procedure and positive-reinforcement operant conditioning techniques were used to measure difference limens (DLs) along a VOT continuum from--70 ms (prevoiced/ba/) to 0 ms (/ba/) to + 70 ms (/pa/). For all contrasts tested, human sensitivity was more acute than that of monkeys. For voicing lag, which spans a phonemic contrast in English, human DLs for a/ba/(standard)-to-/pa/ (target) continuum averaged 8.3 ms compared to 17 ms for monkeys. Human DLs for a/pa/-to-/ba/ continuum averaged 11 ms compared to 25 ms for monkeys. Larger species differences occurred for voicing lead, which is phonemically nondistinctive in English. Human DLs for a /ba/-to-prevoiced/ba/ continuum averaged 8.2 ms and were four times lower than monkeys (35 ms). Monkeys did not reliably discriminate prevoiced /ba/-to-/ba/, whereas humans DLs averaged 18 ms. The effects of eliminating cues in the English VOT contrasts were also examined. Removal of the aspiration noise in /pa/ greatly increased the DLs and reaction times for both humans and monkeys, but straightening out the F1 transition in /ba/ had only minor effects. Results suggest that quantitative differences in sensitivity should be considered when using monkeys to model the psychoacoustic level of human speech perception. 相似文献
18.
This paper proposes a human visual system based data hiding method with the consideration of the local complexity in images. It is known that human vision is more sensitive to the changes in smooth area than that of complex area, we embed less data into blocks with low complexity and embed more data into blocks with rich texture. We use the modified diamond encoding (MDE) as the embedding technique, and employ a sophisticated pixel pair adjustment process to maintain the complexity consistency of blocks before and after embedding data bits. Since the proposed method is robust to LSB-based steganalysis, it is more secure than other existing methods using the LSB replacement as their embedding technique. The experimental results revealed that the proposed method not only offers a better embedding performance, but is also secure under the attack of the LSB based steganalysis tools. 相似文献
19.
Anisotropic diffusion in the nervous system is most commonly modeled by apparent diffusion tensor, which is based on regular diffusion theory. However, the departure of diffusion-induced signal attenuation from a mono-exponential form implies that there is anomalous diffusion. Recently, a novel diffusion NMR theory based on the fractional motion (FM) model, which is an anomalous diffusion model, has been proposed. While the FM model has been applied to both healthy subjects and tumor patients, its anisotropy in the nervous system remains elusive. In this study, this issue was addressed by measuring the FM-related parameters in 12 non-collinear directions. A metric to quantify the directional deviation was derived. Furthermore, the FM-related parameters were modeled as tensors and analyzed in analogy with the conventional diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Experimental results, which were obtained for 15 healthy subjects at 3T, exhibited pronounced anisotropy of the FM-related parameters, although the effects were smaller than the apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC). The tensorial nature for α, which is the Noah exponent in the FM model, showed behavior similar to the ADC, especially the principal eigenvector for α aligned with the dominant white matter fiber directions. The Hurst exponent H in the FM model, however, showed no correlation with the major fiber directions. The anisotropy of the FM model may provide complementary information to DTI and may have potential for tractography and detecting brain abnormalities. 相似文献
20.
Keefe DH Gorga MP Jesteadt W Smith LM 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》2008,123(3):1504-1512
In 2004, Sininger and Cone-Wesson examined asymmetries in the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of otoacoustic emissions (OAE) in infants, reporting that distortion-product (DP)OAE SNR was larger in the left ear, whereas transient-evoked (TE)OAE SNR was larger in the right. They proposed that cochlear and brainstem asymmetries facilitate development of brain-hemispheric specialization for sound processing. Similarly, in 2006 Sininger and Cone-Wesson described ear asymmetries mainly favoring the right ear in infant auditory brainstem responses (ABRs). The present study analyzed 2640 infant responses to further explore these effects. Ear differences in OAE SNR, signal, and noise were evaluated separately and across frequencies (1.5, 2, 3, and 4 kHz), and ABR asymmetries were compared with cochlear asymmetries. Analyses of ear-canal reflectance and admittance showed that asymmetries in middle-ear functioning did not explain cochlear and brainstem asymmetries. Current results are consistent with earlier studies showing right-ear dominance for TEOAE and ABR. Noise levels were higher in the right ear for OAEs and ABRs, causing ear asymmetries in SNR to differ from those in signal level. No left-ear dominance for DPOAE signal was observed. These results do not support a theory that ear asymmetries in cochlear processing mimic hemispheric brain specialization for auditory processing. 相似文献