首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
A series of experiments investigated the effects of continuous broadband noise (ipsilateral) on wave V of the click-evoked brainstem auditory evoked response (BAER). In general, a broadband noise masker increases the latency and decreases the amplitude of wave V. Varying both click and noise intensity, it was found that noise levels above about 40 dB SPL increase the latency and decrease the amplitude of wave V, regardless of click intensity. The effects of noise on wave V amplitude appear constant across click intensity, whereas the effects of a constant noise level on wave V latency decrease at higher click intensities. Both masking and adaptation increase wave V latency, but their combined effects are occlusive: rate-induced wave V latency shift decreases in the presence of continuous broadband noise. The clinical and theoretical implications of these findings are discussed.  相似文献   

2.
The effects of masking noise on wave V of the brain-stem auditory-evoked response (BAER) obtained to pseudorandom pulse sequences are evaluated in two experiments. In the first experiment, the level of broadband noise was covaried with minimum pulse interval (rate) using maximum length sequence analysis (MLSA). Both increasing noise level and decreasing minimum pulse interval decrease wave V amplitude and increase wave V latency. A nonadditivity of rate and noise level was observed such that, at the shortest interpulse intervals, simultaneous background noise produced virtually no latency change and minimal amplitude change, for the noise levels tested. In a second experiment, high-pass masking was performed to assess the feasibility of derived-band techniques using maximum length sequence analysis (MLSA) and to compare the frequency regions responsible for the BAER using MLSA versus conventional averaging. Results of experiment 2 showed that reliable responses across high-pass masker cutoff frequency could be obtained in normal-hearing listeners. The frequency specificity of the MLSA-based responses was nearly identical to that obtained by conventional averaging, although both amplitude and latency of wave V were affected by the high-pass masker cutoff and minimum pulse interval values. These studies suggest that the neuronal populations and frequency regions responsible for the BAER are virtually the same for MLSA and conventional averaging.  相似文献   

3.
A series of experiments evaluated the effects of broadband noise (ipsilateral) on wave V of the brainstem auditory evoked response (BAER) elicited by tone bursts or clicks in the presence of high-pass masking noise. Experiment 1 used 1000- and 4000-Hz, 60-dB nHL tone bursts in the presence of broadband noise. With increasing noise level, wave V latency shift was greater for the 1000-Hz tone bursts, while amplitude decrements were similar for both tone-burst frequencies. Experiment 2 varied high-pass masker cutoff frequency and the level of subtotal masking in the presence of 50-dB nHL clicks. The effects of subtotal masking on wave V (increase in latency and decrease in amplitude) increased with increasing derived-band frequency. Experiment 3 covaried high-pass masker cutoff frequency and subtotal masking level for 1000- and 4000-Hz tone-burst stimuli. The effect of subtotal masking on wave V latency was reduced for both tone-burst frequencies when the response-generating region of the cochlear partition was limited by high-pass maskers. The results of these three experiments suggest that most of the wave V latency shift associated with increasing levels of broadband noise is mediated by a place mechanism when the stimulus is a moderate intensity (60 dB nHL), low-frequency (1000 Hz) tone burst. However, the interpretation of the latency shifts produced by broadband noise for 4000-Hz tone-burst stimuli is made more complex by multiple technical factors discussed herein.  相似文献   

4.
Experiments were conducted to assess the contribution of place mechanisms to the effect of rise--fall time on wave V of the human brainstem auditory evoked response (BAER). Noise bursts of 4- and 10-ms duration were presented at various rise-fall times (0, 1, 2, and 5 ms). Subtractive high-pass masking techniques were used to determine the effect of rise time as a function of derived-band frequency. In general, increasing rise time prolonged wave V latency but did not affect amplitude. Rise-time effects did not depend on derived-band frequency and similar effects were seen in the unmasked conditions. In addition, narrowing the derived band did not alter the observed effects on latency and amplitude. Signal envelope showed no effects on traveling wave velocity. These results suggest that place mechanisms contribute little to changes in the BAER associated with rise--fall time.  相似文献   

5.
Thresholds for the detection of a 50-ms test stimulus delivered to the thenar eminence were measured as a function of the time interval between the offset of a 500-ms masking stimulus and the onset of the test stimulus (delta t). The frequency of the masker and the test stimulus was the same during a particular testing session and was either 25 or 250 Hz. At all values of delta t, older subjects exhibited significantly more masking than did young subjects. The effects of age were greater for stimuli that primarily affect the Pacinian system (250 Hz) than those that primarily affect non-Pacinian systems (25 Hz). Psychophysical measurements of the apparent duration of tactile sensations suggest that both sensory persistence and adaptation are affected by aging. Since adaptation seemed to be the more dominant factor for stimuli with durations as long as 500 ms, it was concluded that the effects of aging on forward masking seen in our study were due mainly to increased amounts of adaptation produced by the masker.  相似文献   

6.
The effects of broadband noise on the brain-stem auditory evoked response (BAER) are reported for two experiments. Experiment 1 used a high-pass subtractive-masking technique and covaried derived bandwidth and continuous broadband noise level. Comparison of responses to half-octave wide derived bands in the presence of within-band noise showed that wave V latency changes were greater than could be explained on the basis of shifts in the cochlear region responsible for generating the response. The magnitude of within-band noise-induced wave V latency shift was independent of the frequency separation of the masker cutoffs. In experiment 2 the effects of noise level and rate on waves I, III, and V of the BAER were evaluated. Peak latencies increased and peak amplitudes decreased with increasing noise level and rate. Higher noise levels and rates produced an increased central (I-V) conduction time in which the wave III-V increase was greater than the wave I-III increase. Together, these results are most consistent with the hypothesis that a nonplace, central auditory mechanism produces most of the noise-induced latency shifts in normal-hearing adults.  相似文献   

7.
The frequency contribution to the click-evoked ABR wave V was examined in adults and 3-month-old infants through the use of notch-filtered broadband noise. Notch center frequencies were set at 1.0, 4.0, and 8.0 kHz. Responses were obtained at 20, 40, and 60 dBnHL during the simultaneous presentation of each notched-noise masker as well as in an unmasked condition. The ABR wave V was analyzed for absolute latency and amplitude, as well as latency and amplitude changes resulting from the introduction of masking. Analyses showed wave V latency and amplitude values to be similar for adults and infants within the 1.0-kHz notch. Differences between adult and infant groups were observed as the notch was shifted to the high frequencies. Further, latency and amplitude shifts resulting from the introduction of masking noise produced differential effects on infant responses when compared to adults.  相似文献   

8.
9.
The simultaneous presentation of a 10- and 10.86-kHz tone produces an 860-Hz cochlear nerve difference tone (DT) response in the gerbil which persists for the duration of the stimulus. Forward masking shows this response is generated by neurons sharply tuned to the stimulus frequencies. When compared with the DT response, the cochlear nerve compound action potential (CAP) to a single tone is smaller in amplitude, has a higher nonmasked threshold, and produces a less sensitive tuning curve (TC). Forward maskers can also produce amplitude enhancement of the CAP, but this was not observed for the onset portion of the DT response. The CAP TC is as sharply tuned as the TC of either the DT onset response or the entire DT response. A comparison was made of tuning of the DT response to the onset, the first half and second half of the 23-ms duration probe stimulus, using either a 5- or 15-ms masker-probe interval. An increase of the tip threshold of the TC to all three portions of the stimulus occurred as the interval was increased between the end of the masker and the midpoint of the portion of the stimulus under question. The 15-ms masker-probe interval produced sharper TCs.  相似文献   

10.
Psychophysical forward-masked thresholds were estimated for 3- and 6-month-old infants and for adults. Listeners detected a repeated 1000-Hz probe, with 16-ms rise time, no steady-state duration, and 16-ms fall time. Unmasked thresholds were determined for one group of listeners who were trained to respond when they heard the probe but not at other times. In the masking conditions, each tone burst was preceded by a 100-ms broadband noise masker at 65 dB SPL. Listeners were trained to respond when they heard the probe and masker, but not when they heard the masker alone. The masker-probe interval, delta t, was either 5, 10, 25, or 200 ms. Four groups of subjects listened in the masked conditions, each at one value of delta t. Each listener attempted to complete a block of 32 trials including four probe levels chosen to span the range of expected thresholds. "Group" thresholds, based on average psychometric functions, as well as thresholds for individual listeners, were estimated. Both group and individual thresholds declined with delta t, as expected, for both infants and adults. Infants' masked thresholds were higher than those of adults, and comparison of masked to unmasked thresholds suggested that infants demonstrate more forward masking than adults, particularly at short delta t. Forward masking appeared to have greater effects on 3-month-olds' detection than on either 6-month-olds' or adults'. Compared to adults, 6-month-olds demonstrated more forward masking only for delta t of 5 ms. Thus, susceptibility to forward masking may be nearly mature by 6 months of age.  相似文献   

11.
The effects of forward masker duration on psychophysical measures of frequency selectivity were investigated in two experiments. In both experiments, masker duration was 50 or 400 ms, signal duration was 20 ms, and there was no delay between masker offset and signal onset. In the first experiment, growth-of-masking functions were measured for a masker whose frequency was below, at, or above the 1000-Hz signal frequency. From those data, input filter patterns (IFPs) were plotted for masker levels from 40-90 dB SPL. In the second experiment, masking patterns (MPs) were measured for a 1000-Hz masker presented at 50, 70, and 90 dB SPL. Both measures of frequency selectivity (IFPs and MPs) indicate that frequency selectivity is greater for the 400-ms masker. These data suggest that there may be a sharpening of frequency selectivity with time at a stage prior to the adaptation observed in forward masking.  相似文献   

12.
When more than one sinusoid is used as a masker, more masking is observed than would be predicted by a simple combination of their individual effects. This masking is dramatically increased when the masker components vary in frequency and intensity with each presentation. These studies manipulated several masker parameters under conditions of high masker uncertainty, examining the effect of excluding critical-band components, fixing or randomizing component amplitudes and frequencies, and narrowing the frequency range of the components. The signal was always a 200-ms, 1000-Hz sinusoid, presented simultaneously with the 200-ms masker. Removing critical-band components reduced the amount of masking, but considerable masking remained that appears to be nonperipheral in origin. Fixing masker frequencies across the two intervals of a trial greatly reduced the masking observed, whereas fixing masker amplitudes had no effect. Reducing the frequency range from 5000 to 2700 Hz generally increased the masking observed, but appeared to depend on other masker parameters. Summaries across ten listeners show individual differences that are resistant to extensive training. It is difficult to account for most of the masking observed in terms of masker energy falling near the region of the signal.  相似文献   

13.
These experiments examine how comodulation masking release (CMR) varies with masker bandwidth, modulator bandwidth, and signal duration. In experiment 1, thresholds were measured for a 400-ms, 2000-Hz signal masked by continuous noise varying in bandwidth from 50-3200 Hz in 1-oct steps. In one condition, using random noise maskers, thresholds increased with increasing bandwidth up to 400 Hz and then remained approximately constant. In another set of conditions, the masker was multiplied (amplitude modulated) by a low-pass noise (bandwidth varied from 12.5-400 Hz in 1-oct steps). This produced correlated envelope fluctuations across frequency. Thresholds were generally lower than for random noise maskers with the same bandwidth. For maskers less than one critical band wide, the release from masking was largest (about 5 dB) for maskers with low rates of modulation (12.5-Hz-wide low-pass modulator). It is argued that this release from masking is not a "true" CMR but results from a within-channel cue. For broadband maskers (greater than 400 Hz), the release from masking increased with increasing masker bandwidth and decreasing modulator bandwidth, reaching an asymptote of 12 dB for a masker bandwidth of 800 Hz and a modulator bandwidth of 50 Hz. Most of this release from masking can be attributed to a CMR. In experiment 2, the modulator bandwidth was fixed at 12.5 Hz and the signal duration was varied. For masker bandwidths greater than 400 Hz, the CMR decreased from 12 to 5 dB as the signal duration was decreased from 400 to 25 ms.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

14.
The overshoot effect can be reduced by temporary hearing loss induced by aspirin or exposure to intense sound. The present study simulated a hearing loss at 4.0 kHz via pure-tone forward masking and examined the effect of the simulation on threshold for a 10-ms, 4.0-kHz signal presented 1 ms after the onset of a 400-ms, broadband noise masker whose spectrum level was 20 dB SPL. Masker frequency was 3.6, 4.0, or 4.2 kHz, and masker level was 80 dB SPL. Subject-dependent delays were determined such that 10 or 20 dB of masking at 4.0 kHz was produced. In general, the pure-tone forward masker did not reduce the simultaneous-masked threshold, suggesting that elevating threshold with a pure-tone forward masker does not sufficiently simulate the effect of a temporary hearing loss on overshoot.  相似文献   

15.
Three experiments evaluating the effects of various stimulus manipulations on the click-evoked gerbil brain-stem auditory-evoked response (BAER) are reported. In experiment 1, click polarity and level were covaried. With increasing click level, there is a parallel decrease in the latency of the first five BAER peaks (i-v) and an increase in BAER peak amplitudes. Mean wave i amplitude was greater for rarefaction than condensation clicks at high click levels; mean wave v amplitude was greater for condensation clicks at higher click levels. Experiment 2 covaried click rate and polarity. The latency of the BAER peaks increased with increasing click repetition rate. This rate-dependent latency increase was greater for the later BAER peaks, resulting in an increase in the i-v interval with increasing click rate. As rate increased, the amplitudes of waves i and v decreased monotonically, whereas the amplitudes of waves ii-iv were largely uninfluenced by click rate. As in experiment 1, mean wave i amplitude was greater for rarefaction clicks, whereas mean wave v amplitude was greater for condensation clicks. The magnitude of these polarity dependencies on waves i and v amplitude decreased with increasing click rate. Experiment 3 evaluated the effects of click polarity on BAERs to high-intensity (100 dB pSPL) clicks presented at a rate of 10 Hz. In eight of ten gerbils evaluated, wave i amplitude was greater to rarefaction clicks, and, in all ten animals, wave v amplitude was greater to condensation clicks. The effects of click level and rate on BAER peak amplitudes, latencies, and interwave intervals are reminiscent of stimulus dependencies reported for the human BAER. The effects of click polarity on the amplitudes of waves i and v of the gerbil BAER have also been reported for the human BAER.  相似文献   

16.
The present study was a follow-up to a pilot study in which it was found that a 500-Hz-wide narrow-band noise (NBN) masker produced more masking than a tonal (T) masker for signal frequencies both above and below the masker frequency. The aim of the present study was to determine to what extent these results were influenced by an interaction of the relatively rapid temporal envelope fluctuations of the NBN and the short (10-ms) duration of the signal. In the first experiment, the masking produced by a regular NBN, a low-noise noise (LNN), and a T was compared. The LNN produced less masking than the NBN, and about as much as the T, suggesting that the inherent amplitude fluctuations in the NBN were largely responsible for the greater masking produced by that masker. In the second experiment, the masking produced by a regular NBN was compared with that by a T for a signal duration of 10 or 200 ms. The difference in masking between the two maskers was reduced or eliminated when the signal duration was 200 ms, because the threshold in the presence of the NBN masker decreased more with increasing signal duration. This could reflect a decreased "confusion" between the signal and the inherent fluctuations of the NBN masker.  相似文献   

17.
Two experiments concerning the effects of masking noise on the gerbil brain-stem auditory-evoked response (BAER) are reported. Experiment 1 evaluated the effects of broadband masking noise on the BAER obtained to condensation and rarefaction clicks. With increasing noise level, there was an increase in BAER peak latencies, an increase in the i-v interval, and a decrease in peak amplitudes. Experiment 2 evaluated the effects of high-pass masking noise on the BAER obtained to condensation and rarefaction clicks. Both high-pass responses and derived-band responses were evaluated. For high-pass responses, with decreasing masker cutoff frequency, there was an increase in BAER peak latencies, a decrease in the i-v interval, and a decrease in peak amplitudes. For derived-band responses, with decreasing derived-band frequency, there was an increase in peak latencies and a decrease in the i-v interval. A comparison of wave i and wave v amplitudes across derived-band frequency demonstrates a greater contribution of high-frequency cochlear regions to wave i than wave v. Small, insignificant, effects of click polarity on BAER peak amplitudes were observed. These trends were in the direction seen in a companion paper [R. Burkard and H. F. Voigt, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 2514-2525 (1989)] and were, in general, reduced by the presence of broadband or high-pass maskers.  相似文献   

18.
The effect of masker-frequency variability on the detection performance of 7-9 month-old infants and adults was examined. Listeners detected a 300-ms 1000-Hz pure tone masked by: (1) A random-frequency two-tone complex; (2) a fixed-frequency two-tone complex; or (3) a broadband noise. Maskers repeated at 300-ms intervals throughout testing at 60 dB SPL. The signal was presented simultaneously with one presentation of the masker. Thresholds were determined adaptively using an observer-based method. Infants' thresholds were higher than adults' in all conditions, but infants' and adults' thresholds changed with masker condition in qualitatively similar ways. The fixed two-tone complex produced masking for both age groups, but more masking for infants than for adults. For infants and adults, the random two-tone complex produced more masking than broadband noise, but the difference was greater for infants than for adults. For infants and adults, the random two-tone complex produced more masking than the fixed two-tone complex, and the difference between these conditions was similar for both age groups. These results suggest that infants are more susceptible to informational masking than adults in the absence of spectral variability. Whether infants are more susceptible to the effects of masker-frequency variability than adults remains to be clarified.  相似文献   

19.
Forward masking: adaptation or integration?   总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8  
The aim of this study was to attempt to distinguish between neural adaptation and persistence (or temporal integration) as possible explanations of forward masking. Thresholds were measured for a sinusoidal signal as a function of signal duration for conditions where the delay between the masker offset and the signal offset (the offset-offset interval) was fixed. The masker was a 200-ms broadband noise, presented at a spectrum level of 40 dB (re: 20 microPa), and the signal was a 4-kHz sinusoid, gated with 2-ms ramps. The offset-offset interval was fixed at various durations between 4 and 102 ms and signal thresholds were measured for a range of signal durations at each interval. A substantial decrease in thresholds was observed with increasing duration for signal durations up to about 20 ms. At short offset-offset intervals, the amount of temporal integration exceeded that normally found in quiet. The results were simulated using models of temporal integration (the temporal-window model) and adaptation. For both models, the inclusion of a peripheral nonlinearity, similar to that observed physiologically in studies of the basilar membrane, was essential in producing a good fit to the data. Both models were about equally successful in accounting for the present data. However, the temporal-window model provided a somewhat better account of similar data from a simultaneous-masking experiment, using the same parameters. This suggests that the linear, time-invariant properties of the temporal-window approach are appropriate for modeling forward masking. Overall the results confirm that forward masking can be described in terms of peripheral nonlinearity followed by linear temporal integration at higher levels in the auditory system. However, the difference in predictions between the adaptation and integration models is relatively small, meaning that influence of adaptation cannot be ruled out.  相似文献   

20.
The temporal course of masking and the auditory filter shape   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Recent experiments have shown that frequency selectivity measured in tone-on-tone simultaneous masking improves with increasing delay of a brief signal relative to the onset of a longer duration gated masker. To determine whether a similar improvement occurs for a notched-noise masker, threshold was measured for a 20-ms signal presented at the beginning, the temporal center, or the end of the 400-ms masker (simultaneous masking), or immediately following the masker (forward masking). The notch width was varied systematically and the notch was placed both symmetrically and asymmetrically about the 1-kHz signal frequency. Growth-of-masking functions were determined for each temporal condition, for a noise masker without a spectral notch. These functions were used to express the thresholds from the notched-noise experiment in terms of the level of a flat-spectrum noise which would produce the same threshold. In simultaneous masking the auditory filter shapes derived from the transformed data did not change significantly with signal delay, suggesting that the selectivity of the auditory filter does not develop over time. In forward masking the auditory filter shapes were sharper than those for simultaneous masking, particularly on the high-frequency side, which was attributed to suppression.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号