首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 281 毫秒
1.
In a companion article [L. I. Hellstrom, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 230-242 (1989)], it was shown that psychophysical pulsation threshold masking patterns (PTPs) for high-pass noise maskers are not a simple transformation of the profile of activity evoked in the auditory nerve by the masker. In this article, PTPs are compared with neural representations in which interactions of masker and probe are considered. It is hypothesized that, at pulsation threshold, some criterion value of rate change occurs when the stimulus switches from masker to probe. The iso-rate probe level, defined for single auditory-nerve fibers, is the probe level at which this rate change is zero. Iso-rate probe levels are lowest when probe frequency equals best frequency (BF) of the fiber. Profiles of iso-rate probe level versus BF (equal to probe frequency) are qualitatively similar to PTPs but differ quantitatively, e.g., in the rate of growth of probe level with masker level (1.2 dB/dB for PTPs, 0.54 dB/dB for iso-rate profiles). Quantitative differences can be further reduced by requiring a positive rate criterion. These results suggest that PTPs are not solely a reflection of the internal representation of the masker, but reflect responses to the probe tone as well.  相似文献   

2.
Cochlear nonlinearity was estimated over a wide range of center frequencies and levels in listeners with normal hearing, using a forward-masking method. For a fixed low-level probe, the masker level required to mask the probe was measured as a function of the masker-probe interval, to produce a temporal masking curve (TMC). TMCs were measured for probe frequencies of 500, 1000, 2000, 4000, and 8000 Hz, and for masker frequencies 0.5, 0.7, 0.9, 1.0 (on frequency), 1.1, and 1.6 times the probe frequency. Across the range of probe frequencies, the TMCs for on-frequency maskers showed two or three segments with clearly distinct slopes. If it is assumed that the rate of decay of the internal effect of the masker is constant across level and frequency, the variations in the slopes of the TMCs can be attributed to variations in cochlear compression. Compression-ratio estimates for on-frequency maskers were between 3:1 and 5:1 across the range of probe frequencies. Compression did not decrease at low frequencies. The slopes of the TMCs for the lowest frequency probe (500 Hz) did not change with masker frequency. This suggests that compression extends over a wide range of stimulus frequencies relative to characteristic frequency in the apical region of the cochlea.  相似文献   

3.
In their recent article "Offset AP masker tuning curve and the FFT of the stimulus" [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 84, 1354-1362 (1988)], Henry and Lewis demonstrated that the tuning curve obtained by the simultaneous masking of the whole nerve action potential (AP) could have two tips when the AP is generated at the offset of the envelope of a high-level probe. The primary tip falls below the probe frequency, whereas the secondary tip falls above the probe frequency. Curves obtained for the onset response with either forward or simultaneous masking did not show the secondary peak, nor did curves obtained for the offset response with forward masking. Henry and Lewis discussed various reasons for the secondary tip, but came to no conclusion as to the underlying mechanisms. Here, it is reasoned that the secondary tip of the offset curve can be simply explained by the generation within the cochlea of intermodulation distortion (IMD), which acts as a forward masker to the offset response. The IMD is dominated by the cubic component (2f1-f2) and arises from the interaction of the probe tone and the simultaneous masker. Finally, it is reasoned that the lower sideband of the frequency splatter present at probe offset is the primary stimulus for the evoked neural response under probe offset conditions. Thus the offset curve will always have a primary tip that is lower in frequency than that of the respective onset curve. These hypotheses are supported by single-fiber data.  相似文献   

4.
Across-critical-band processing of amplitude-modulated tones   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Two experiments using two-tone sinusoidally amplitude-modulated stimuli were conducted to assess cross-channel effects in processing low-frequency amplitude modulation. In experiment I, listeners were asked to discriminate between two sets of two-tone amplitude-modulated complexes. In one set, the modulation phase of the lower frequency carrier tone was different from that of the upper frequency carrier tone. In the other stimulus set, both amplitude-modulated carriers had the same modulator phase. The amount of phase shift required to discriminate between the two stimulus sets was determined as a function of the separation between the two carriers, modulation depth, and modulation frequency. Listeners could discriminate a 50 degrees-60 degrees phase shift between the modulated envelopes for tones separated by more than a critical band. In experiment II, the modulation depth required to detect modulation of a probe carrier was measured in the presence of an amplitude-modulated masker. The threshold for detecting probe modulation was determined as a function of the separation between the masker and probe carriers, the phase difference between the masker and probe modulators, and masker modulation depth (in all conditions, the rate of probe and masker modulation was 10 Hz). The threshold for detecting probe modulation was raised substantially when the masker tone was also modulated. The results are consistent with theories suggesting that amplitude modulation helps form auditory objects from complex sound fields.  相似文献   

5.
It is now undisputed that the best frequency (BF) of basal basilar-membrane (BM) sites shifts downwards as the stimulus level increases. The direction of the shift for apical sites is, by contrast, less well established. Auditory nerve studies suggest that the BF shifts in opposite directions for apical and basal BM sites with increasing stimulus level. This study attempts to determine if this is the case in humans. Psychophysical tuning curves (PTCs) were measured using forward masking for probe frequencies of 125, 250, 500, and 6000 Hz. The level of a masker tone required to just mask a fixed low-level probe tone was measured for different masker-probe time intervals. The duration of the intervals was adjusted as necessary to obtain PTCs for the widest possible range of masker levels. The BF was identified from function fits to the measured PTCs and it almost always decreased with increasing level. This result is inconsistent with most auditory-nerve observations obtained from other mammals. Several explanations are discussed, including that it may be erroneous to assume that low-frequency PTCs reflect the tuning of apical BM sites exclusively and that the inherent frequency response of the inner hair cell may account for the discrepancy.  相似文献   

6.
The potential for interactions between steady-state evoked responses to simultaneous auditory stimuli was investigated in two bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Three experiments were conducted using either a probe stimulus (probe condition) or a probe in the presence of a masker (probe-plus-masker condition). In the first experiment, the probe and masker were sinusoidal amplitude-modulated (SAM) tones. Probe and masker frequencies and masker level were manipulated to provide variable masking conditions. Probe frequencies were 31.7, 63.5, 100.8, and 127.0 kHz. The second experiment was identical to the first except only the 63.5 kHz probe was used and maskers were pure tones. For the third experiment, thresholds were measured for the probe and probe-plus-masker conditions using two techniques, one based on the lowest detectable response and the other based on a regression analysis. Results demonstrated localized masking effects where lower frequency maskers suppressed higher frequency probes and higher amplitude maskers produced a greater masking effect. The pattern of pure tone masking was nearly identical to SAM tone masking. The two threshold estimates were similar in low masking conditions, but in high masking conditions the lowest detectable response tended to overestimate thresholds while the regression-based analysis tended to underestimate thresholds.  相似文献   

7.
Three psychophysical forward masking studies were conducted on a multichannel cochlear implant patient. The first study investigated the masking pattern produced by a bipolar electrode pair at different stimulus currents. It was found that the masking pattern for a single-masker bipolar electrode pair had a maximum located at an electrode position where the masker and probe coincided. The spread of the masking pattern was not symmetrical about the maximum. The amount of masking decreased very rapidly toward the apical direction and less rapidly toward the basal direction from the position of the maximum. As the stimulus current increased, the amount of masking at the maximum increased and the masking pattern broadened toward the base. The second study investigated the masking pattern produced by the activation of single bipolar electrode pairs with different spatial extents. The spatial extent of a bipolar electrode pair is defined as the distance between the apical and basal electrode members of the bipolar pair. With a small spatial extent (1.5 mm), the more basal electrode pairs (higher threshold and smaller dynamic range) produced broader masking patterns than the more apical electrode pairs (lower threshold and wider dynamic range), suggesting that there was more current spread at the basal region. With a larger spatial extent (4.5 mm), an additional secondary masking maximum was observed in the vicinity of the apical electrode member of the masker; this was observed only when the apical electrode member lay within the low-threshold apical region. The third study investigated the masking patterns produced by two loudness balanced bipolar masker electrode pairs activated within a stimulus period (inverse of the pulse repetition rate). The biphasic current pulses delivered to the two electrode pairs were nonoverlapping in time. It was found that, at any probe electrode position, the amount of masking produced by the two combined bipolar electrode pairs approximately followed the greater of the two maskings produced respectively by the two individual bipolar masker electrode pairs.  相似文献   

8.
Forward-masked psychophysical tuning curves were obtained from normal-hearing listeners at different probe levels in quiet and in a broadband background noise. In quiet, tuning-curve shape changed with probe level. For six listeners, tuning curves became broader with increasing probe level, primarily due to a decrease in the low-frequency slopes. For one listener, tuning curves became narrower with increasing probe level. The addition of a background noise, which was presented continuously at a level 10 dB below the noise level required to mask the probe tone, reduced the masker levels required to mask the probe tone. The reduction was greater near the tip of the tuning curve than on the tail, so that tuning curves in background noise were narrower than those obtained in quiet. Tuning curves with comparable masker levels near the tip of the tuning curve (Lmtip) were similar in shape, regardless of probe level or whether tuning curves were obtained in quiet or noise. Comparisons of tuning-curve characteristics derived by fitting tuning curves with least-squares procedures, indicated that low-frequency slopes decreased with Lmtip. As a consequence, Q10 dB values decreased with Lmtip. These results are consistent with the interpretation that tuning-curve shapes are determined by the intensities of the maskers required to mask the probe tone. The addition of a background noise restricted (partially masked) the excitation pattern of the probe so that lower masker intensities were required to "forward mask" the probe tone, and narrower tuning curves resulted from less intense markers. The results are well described by a two-process model of auditory excitation patterns.  相似文献   

9.
This study describes a method for recording the electrically evoked, whole-nerve action potential (EAP) in users of the Ineraid cochlear implant. The method is an adaptation of one originally used by Charlet de Sauvage et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 73, 615-627 (1983)] in guinea pigs. The response, recorded from 11 subjects, consists of a single negative peak that occurs with a latency of approximately 0.4 ms. EAP input/output functions are steeply sloping and monotonic. Response amplitudes ranging up to 160 micro V have been recorded. Slope of the EAP input/output function correlates modestly (approximately 0.6-0.69) with results of tests measuring word recognition skills. The refractory properties of the auditory nerve were also assessed. Differences across subjects were found in the rate of recovery from the refractory state. These findings imply that there may be difference across subjects in the accuracy with which rapid temporal cues can be coded at the level of the auditory nerve. Reasonably strong correlations (approximately 0.74-0.85) have been found between the magnitude of the slope of these recovery curves and performance on tests of word recognition.  相似文献   

10.
This study examined whether "modulation masking" could be produced by temporal similarity of the probe and masker envelopes, even when the masker envelope did not contain a spectral component close to the probe frequency. Both masker and probe amplitude modulation were applied to a single 4-kHz sinusoidal or narrow-band noise carrier with a level of 70 dB SPL. The threshold for detecting 5-Hz probe modulation was affected by the presence of a pair of masker modulators beating at a 5-Hz rate (40 and 45 Hz, 50 and 55 Hz, or 60 and 65 Hz). The threshold was dependent on the phase of the probe modulation relative to the beat cycle of the masker modulators; the threshold elevation was greatest (12-15 dB for the sinusoidal carrier and 9-11 dB for the noise carrier, expressed as 20 log m) when the peak amplitude of the probe modulation coincided with a peak in the beat cycle. The maximum threshold elevation of the 5-Hz probe produced by the beating masker modulators was 7-12 dB greater than that produced by the individual components of the masker modulators. The threshold elevation produced by the beating masker modulators was 2-10 dB greater for 5-Hz probe modulation than for 3- or 7-Hz probe modulation. These results cannot be explained in terms of the spectra of the envelopes of the stimuli, as the beating masker modulators did not produce a 5-Hz component in the spectra of the envelopes. The threshold for detecting 5-Hz probe modulation in the presence of 5-Hz masker modulation varied with the relative phase of the probe and masker modulation. The pattern of results was similar to that found with the beating two-component modulators, except that thresholds were highest when the masker and probe were 180 degrees out of phase. The results are consistent with the idea that nonlinearities within the auditory system introduce distortion in the internal representation of the envelopes of the stimuli. In the case of two-component beating modulators, a weak component is introduced at the beat rate, and it has an amplitude minimum when the beat cycle is at its maximum. The results could be fitted well using two models, one based on the concept of a sliding temporal integrator and one based on the concept of a modulation filter bank.  相似文献   

11.
Forward-masking growth functions for on-frequency (6-kHz) and off-frequency (3-kHz) sinusoidal maskers were measured in quiet and in a high-pass noise just above the 6-kHz probe frequency. The data show that estimates of response-growth rates obtained from those functions in quiet, which have been used to infer cochlear compression, are strongly dependent on the spread of probe excitation toward higher frequency regions. Therefore, an alternative procedure for measuring response-growth rates was proposed, one that employs a fixed low-level probe and avoids level-dependent spread of probe excitation. Fixed-probe-level temporal masking curves (TMCs) were obtained from normal-hearing listeners at a test frequency of 1 kHz, where the short 1-kHz probe was fixed in level at about 10 dB SL. The level of the preceding forward masker was adjusted to obtain masked threshold as a function of the time delay between masker and probe. The TMCs were obtained for an on-frequency masker (1 kHz) and for other maskers with frequencies both below and above the probe frequency. From these measurements, input/output response-growth curves were derived for individual ears. Response-growth slopes varied from >1.0 at low masker levels to <0.2 at mid masker levels. In three subjects, response growth increased again at high masker levels (>80 dB SPL). For the fixed-level probe, the TMC slopes changed very little in the presence of a high-pass noise masking upward spread of probe excitation. A greater effect on the TMCs was observed when a high-frequency cueing tone was used with the masking tone. In both cases, however, the net effects on the estimated rate of response growth were minimal.  相似文献   

12.
Responses of chinchilla auditory-nerve fibers were measured for stimulus conditions analogous to those in which psychophysical release from masking has been observed in humans. The maskers were two equal power, narrow-band noise stimuli with different amplitude envelopes. The neurons in the sample fell into three groups that resolved the maskers' envelopes with varying degrees of accuracy. The boundaries of these groups were not sharply delineated by characteristic frequency (CF) but were dependent on the relationship between the masker level and the neurons' thresholds at the masker frequency. For the neurons that best preserved the maskers' envelope fluctuations, a neural release from masking was observed; rate-based neural masked thresholds were higher for the masker with the least fluctuating envelope. The results suggest that neural and psychophysical release from masking arises because the probe evokes larger rate changes, relative to the background response to the masker, during periods of low masker energy. Between two otherwise equivalent maskers, the one with the periods of lowest energy will produce the lower masked thresholds because rate changes are larger and more detectable.  相似文献   

13.
Research on informational masking for nonspeech stimuli has focused on the effects of spectral uncertainty in the masker. In this letter, results are presented from some preliminary probe experiments in which the spectrum of the masker is held fixed but the spatial properties of the masker are randomized. In addition, in some tests, the overall level of the stimulus is randomized. These experiments differ from previous experiments that have measured the effect of spatial uncertainty on masking in that the only attributes (aside from level) that distinguish the target from the masker are the spatial attributes; in all of the tests, the target and masker were statistically identical, statistically independent, narrowband noise signals. In general, the results indicate that detection performance is degraded by spatial uncertainty in the masker but that compared both to the effects of spectral uncertainty and to the effects of overall-level uncertainty, the effects of spatial uncertainty are relatively small.  相似文献   

14.
Previous data on the masking level difference (MLD) have suggested that NoSpi detection for a long-duration signal is dominated by signal energy occurring in masker envelope minima. This finding was expanded upon using a brief 500-Hz tonal signal that coincided with either the envelope maximum or minimum of a narrow-band Gaussian noise masker centered at 500 Hz, and data were collected at a range of masker levels. Experiment 1 employed a typical MLD stimulus, consisting of a 30-ms signal and a 50-Hz-wide masker with abrupt spectral edges, and experiment 2 used stimuli generated to eliminate possible spectral cues. Results were quite similar for the two types of stimuli. At the highest masker level the MLD for signals coinciding with masker envelope minima was substantially larger than that for signals coinciding with envelope maxima, a result that was primarily due to decreased NoSpi thresholds in masker minima. For most observers this effect was greatly reduced or eliminated at the lowest masker level. These level effects are broadly consistent with the presence of physiological background noise and with a level-dependent binaural temporal window. Comparison of these results with predictions of a published model suggest that basilar-membrane compression alone does not account for this level effect.  相似文献   

15.
To discriminate between broadband noises with and without a high-frequency spectral notch is more difficult at 70-80 dB sound pressure level than at lower or higher levels [Alves-Pinto, A. and Lopez-Poveda, E. A. (2005). "Detection of high-frequency spectral notches as a function of level," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 118, 2458-2469]. One possible explanation is that the notch is less clearly represented internally at 70-80 dB SPL than at any other level. To test this hypothesis, forward-masking patterns were measured for flat-spectrum and notched noise maskers for masker levels of 50, 70, 80, and 90 dB SPL. Masking patterns were measured in two conditions: (1) fixing the masker-probe time interval at 2 ms and (2) varying the interval to achieve similar masked thresholds for different masker levels. The depth of the spectral notch remained approximately constant in the fixed-interval masking patterns and gradually decreased with increasing masker level in the variable-interval masking patterns. This difference probably reflects the effects of peripheral compression. These results are inconsistent with the nonmonotonic level-dependent performance in spectral discrimination. Assuming that a forward-masking pattern is a reasonable psychoacoustical correlate of the auditory-nerve rate-profile representation of the stimulus spectrum, these results undermine the common view that high-frequency spectral notches must be encoded in the rate-profile of auditory-nerve fibers.  相似文献   

16.
Previous action potential (AP) tuning curve methods have used a reduction in amplitude of the probe-elicited AP as an indication of tone-induced masking. The reduction criterion used in different studies has varied from 25% to 100%. For low level probe stimuli, which elicit a low-amplitude AP, this is a sensitive indicator. In contrast, for high-amplitude AP responses elicited by high-level stimuli, the required reduction in absolute terms is large, making it an insensitive indicator. AP tuning curves have been recorded using a sensitive method for detecting masker/probe interaction with a fixed criterion, unrelated to the unmasked AP amplitude. For each masking condition, a derived response was obtained by digitally subtracting the tone-masked AP waveform from the unmasked response. Derived responses are generated if there are ANY changes in the AP waveform induced by the masker, including amplitude changes, latency changes, or even changes in AP morphology not necessarily associated with the major peaks. A fixed criterion (10 microV) of tone-derived (TD) response was used as an indication of interaction of the responses to the masker and probe. Tuning curves generated by this method were compared with those generated by conventional amplitude reduction (AR) methods. TD tuning curves show different characteristics, especially with respect to increasing probe levels. They appear to give a good representation of the array of afferent fibers responding to a probe stimulus. In addition, frequency regions making minor contributions to the AP are better represented in TD tuning curves.  相似文献   

17.
This study examined the time course of adaptation and recovery from adaptation of single auditory-nerve fiber responses. The conditions studied were: (1) adaptation response using low level, 800 Hz or characteristic frequency (CF) stimuli; and (2) onset recovery and whole tone response recovery of a probe tone following a masker of equal frequency with variable silent intervals between the masker offset and probe onset. Single unit responses to 290 ms long, 800 Hz or CF tones presented at 10-30 dB SL were recorded from the auditory nerve of the cat. Adaptation properties were determined and fit to the equation: A(tp) = Yre(-tp/tau Rr) + Yse(-tp/tau Rs) + Ass. Recovery from adaptation was determined by recording the response of a probe tone following a 100-ms masker tone equal in frequency to the probe, and with amplitudes ranging from 20- to 30-dB relative to the probe amplitude. Both the onset recovery and the whole tone recovery were determined for the single unit responses. The onset data were analyzed and fit to either the equation: A (delta xt,tp) = Ass - Yre(-tp/tau Rr) - Yse(- delta t/tau Rs) or A (delta t,tp) = Ass - Yre(- delta t/tau R). The whole tone response showed two distinctive time patterns that could be fit to either an adaptation equation or to the two-time-constant recovery equation, depending on the relative amplitude of the masker and the length of the silent interval between masker offset and probe onset. The results of this study indicate that single fiber time constants are comparable to those measured in previous studies using the auditory-nerve neurophonic (ANN). Likewise, the pattern of recovery of the whole tone response for single fiber responses is comparable to the ANN. Possible sites and mechanisms for adaptation and recovery from adaptation taking into account recent data from electrical stimulation studies and receptor channel morphology and kinetics are discussed.  相似文献   

18.
Psychophysical single-pulse forward-masking (SPFM) recovery functions were measured for three electrodes in each of eight subjects with the nucleus mini-22 cochlear implant. Masker and probe stimuli were single 200-micros/phase biphasic current pulses. Recovery functions were measured at several masker levels spanning the electric dynamic range of electrodes chosen from the apical, middle, and basal regions of each subject's electrode array. Recovery functions were described by an exponential process in which threshold shift (in microA) decreased exponentially with increasing time delay between the masker and probe pulses. Two recovery processes were observed: An initial, rapid-recovery process with an average time constant of 5.5 ms was complete by about 10 ms. A second, slow-recovery process involved less masking than the rapid-recovery process but encompassed much longer time delays, sometimes as long as several hundred milliseconds. Growth-of-masking slopes for the rapid process depended upon time delay, as expected in an exponential recovery process. Unity slopes were observed at a time delay of 0 ms, whereas progressively shallower slopes were observed at time delays of 2 ms and 5 ms. Many recovery functions demonstrated nonmonotonicities or "facilitation" at very short masker-probe delays (1-2 ms). Such nonmonotonicities were usually most pronounced at low masker levels. Time constants for the rapid-recovery process did not vary systematically with masker level or with electrode location along the implanted array. Most subjects demonstrated rapid-recovery time constants less than 7 ms; however, the subject with the longest duration of deafness prior to implantation exhibited clearly prolonged time constants (9-24 ms). Time constants obtained on basal electrodes were inversely related to word recognition scores.  相似文献   

19.
Under certain conditions, speech recognition in noise decreases above conversational levels when signal-to-noise ratio is held constant. The current study was undertaken to determine if nonlinear growth of masking and the subsequent reduction in "effective" signal-to-noise ratio accounts for this decline. Nine young adults with normal hearing listened to monosyllabic words at three levels in each of three levels of a masker shaped to match the speech spectrum. An additional low-level noise equated audibility by producing equivalent masked thresholds for all subjects. If word recognition was determined entirely by signal-to-noise ratio and was independent of overall speech and masker levels, scores at a given signal-to-noise ratio should remain constant with increasing level. Masked pure-tone thresholds measured in the speech-shaped maskers increased linearly with increasing masker level at lower frequencies but nonlinearly at higher frequencies, consistent with nonlinear growth of upward spread of masking that followed the peaks in the spectrum of the speech-shaped masker. Word recognition declined significantly with increasing level when signal-to-noise ratio was held constant which was attributed to nonlinear growth of masking and reduced "effective" signal-to-noise ratio at high speech-shaped masker levels, as indicated by audibility estimates based on the Articulation Index.  相似文献   

20.
Psychometric functions (PFs) for forward-masked tones were obtained for conditions in which signal level was varied to estimate threshold at several masker levels (variable-signal condition), and in which masker level was varied to estimate threshold at several signal levels (variable-masker condition). The changes in PF slope across combinations of masker frequency, masker level, and signal delay were explored in three experiments. In experiment 1, a 2-kHz, 10-ms tone was masked by a 50, 70 or 90 dB SPL, 20-ms on-frequency forward masker, with signal delays of 2, 20, or 40 ms, in a variable-signal condition. PF slopes decreased in conditions where signal threshold was high. In experiments 2 and 3, the signal was a 4-kHz, 10-ms tone, and the masker was either a 4- or 2.4-kHz, 200-ms tone. In experiment 2, on-frequency maskers were presented at 30 to 90 dB SPL in 10-dB steps and off-frequency maskers were presented at 60 to 90 dB SPL in 10-dB steps, with signal delays of 0, 10, or 30 ms, in a variable-signal condition. PF slopes decreased as signal level increased, and this trend was similar for on- and off-frequency maskers. In experiment 3, variable-masker conditions with on- and off-frequency maskers and 0-ms signal delay were presented. In general, the results were consistent with the hypothesis that peripheral nonlinearity is reflected in the PF slopes. The data also indicate that masker level plays a role independent of signal level, an effect that could be accounted for by assuming greater internal noise at higher stimulus levels.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号