共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 11 毫秒
1.
Gutschalk A Micheyl C Oxenham AJ 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》2008,123(2):935-945
Prolonged listening to a pulse train with repetition rates around 100 Hz induces a striking aftereffect, whereby subsequently presented sounds are heard with an unusually "metallic" timbre [Rosenblith et al., Science 106, 333-335 (1947)]. The mechanisms responsible for this auditory aftereffect are currently unknown. Whether the aftereffect is related to an alteration of the perception of temporal envelope fluctuations was evaluated. Detection thresholds for sinusoidal amplitude modulation (AM) imposed onto noise-burst carriers were measured for different AM frequencies (50-500 Hz), following the continuous presentation of a periodic pulse train, a temporally jittered pulse train, or an unmodulated noise. AM detection thresholds for AM frequencies of 100 Hz and above were significantly elevated compared to thresholds in quiet, following the presentation of the pulse-train inducers, and both induced a subjective auditory aftereffect. Unmodulated noise, which produced no audible aftereffect, left AM detection thresholds unchanged. Additional experiments revealed that, like the Rosenblith et al. aftereffect, the effect on AM thresholds does not transfer across ears, is not eliminated by protracted training, and can last several tens of seconds. The results suggest that the Rosenblith et al. aftereffect is related to a temporary alteration in the perception of fast temporal envelope fluctuations in sounds. 相似文献
2.
The temporal course of masking and the auditory filter shape 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
B C Moore P W Poon S P Bacon B R Glasberg 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1987,81(6):1873-1880
Recent experiments have shown that frequency selectivity measured in tone-on-tone simultaneous masking improves with increasing delay of a brief signal relative to the onset of a longer duration gated masker. To determine whether a similar improvement occurs for a notched-noise masker, threshold was measured for a 20-ms signal presented at the beginning, the temporal center, or the end of the 400-ms masker (simultaneous masking), or immediately following the masker (forward masking). The notch width was varied systematically and the notch was placed both symmetrically and asymmetrically about the 1-kHz signal frequency. Growth-of-masking functions were determined for each temporal condition, for a noise masker without a spectral notch. These functions were used to express the thresholds from the notched-noise experiment in terms of the level of a flat-spectrum noise which would produce the same threshold. In simultaneous masking the auditory filter shapes derived from the transformed data did not change significantly with signal delay, suggesting that the selectivity of the auditory filter does not develop over time. In forward masking the auditory filter shapes were sharper than those for simultaneous masking, particularly on the high-frequency side, which was attributed to suppression. 相似文献
3.
S. Gu Y. Jin P. Chen C. Yan E. Della Torre L.H. Bennett 《Physica B: Condensed Matter》2012,407(9):1372-1376
For magnetic materials that display slow decay rates, the entire relaxation process cannot be observed experimentally within a reasonable time interval. While magnetic aftereffect behavior has been understood and analyzed quantitatively in general, the important parameters of magnetic materials with slow decay cannot be easily extracted. We present comprehensive experimental characterization of time, field, and temperature dependence of magnetic properties of a magnetic nanocomposite, which exhibits slow decay. Based on the measurement results and previously developed Preisach–Arrhenius model, we propose an analytical model that can predict the shape of the entire aftereffect curves at different temperatures with only the major hysteresis loop and one aftereffect curve at a specific holding field within the region of interest. The model is validated with both simulated data and measured data. This model allows the derivation of an analytical formula for the time variation of the magnetization based on a Gaussian distribution. 相似文献
4.
The auditory continuity phenomenon occurs when listening to a series of alternating high- and low-level tones: instead of perceiving this intermittence, listeners often report hearing a continuous tone upon which is superimposed a series of intermittent tones. The temporal limits to the perception of this phenomenon are investigated, as well as effects of task instructions on it. A loudness-matching paradigm developed previously [McAdams et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 130, 1580-1591 (1998)] provided both an objective indication of this phenomenon and an indication of its strength. In the studies reported here, the phenomenon was observed reliably when the low-level tones were at least half the duration of the high-level tones: the greater the duration ratio between low-level and high-level tones, the stronger the phenomenon. Duty-cycle duration did not affect the strength of this phenomenon. It proved to be particularly robust as its strength was unaffected by task instructions, level of expertise, and repetition. A model is proposed in which adjusted levels depend on the relative levels of the high- and low-level tones weighted by relative duration and attentional focusing. 相似文献
5.
6.
听神经发放时序在听信息编码中起重要作用,要认识和运用听觉系统对听神经发放时序特征的利用机制必须对听神经发放时序特征有认识,但是已有的随机过程数学模型均不能很好地反映听神经发放时序特征。而关于发放的生理模型,如Hodgkin-Huxley方程、Meddis模型对发放过程的描述均是决定性的、连续性的,不能反映生理过程的离散性和发放过程的随机性。本文基于神经发放生理过程,通过对神经发放条件以及自发发放时神经递质释放时序的描述,建立了一个简单自发发放模型。模型在很好地反映听神经自发发放时序外部统计特征的同时,所得到的神经递质的释放速率在生理上也是合理的。模型还能对纯音激励时听神经的锁相发放做出定性解释,可作为进一步研究反映时序特征的有激励发放模型的基础。 相似文献
7.
8.
P J Fitzgibbons S Gordon-Salant 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》2001,109(6):2955-2963
This study examined age-related changes in temporal sensitivity to increments in the inter-onset intervals (IOI) of successive components in tonal sequences. Temporal discrimination was examined using reference stimulus patterns consisting of five 50-ms, 4000-Hz components with equal tonal IOIs selected from the range 100-600 ms. Discrimination was examined in separate conditions by measuring the relative difference limen (DL) for increments of tonal IOI in comparison sequences. In some conditions, comparison sequences featured equal increments of all tonal lOIs to examined listener sensitivity to uniform changes of sequence rate, or tempo. Other conditions measured the DL for increments of a single target IOI within otherwise uniform-rate comparison sequences. For these measurements, the single target IOI was either fixed in sequence location, or randomized in location across listening trials. Listeners in the study included four groups of young and elderly adults with and without high-frequency hearing loss. The results for all listeners showed the relative DL for rate discrimination to decrease from a maximum at the 100-ms IOI to a smaller stable value across the range of longer sequence IOI. All listeners also exhibited larger relative DLs for discrimination of single target intervals compared to rate discrimination for equivalent reference IOI values. Older listeners showed poorer performance than younger listeners in all conditions, with the largest age differences observed for discrimination of brief single intervals that were varied randomly in sequence location. None of the results revealed significant effects of hearing loss on performance of younger and older listeners. 相似文献
9.
Féron FX Frissen I Boissinot J Guastavino C 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》2010,128(6):3703-3714
Three experiments are reported, which investigated the auditory velocity thresholds beyond which listeners are no longer able to perceptually resolve a smooth circular trajectory. These thresholds were measured for band-limited noises, white noise, and harmonic sounds (HS), and in different acoustical environments. Experiments 1 and 2 were conducted in an acoustically dry laboratory. Observed thresholds varied as a function of stimulus type and spectral content. Thresholds for band-limited noises were unaffected by center frequency and equal to that of white noise. For HS, however, thresholds decreased as the fundamental frequency of the stimulus increased. The third experiment was a replication of the second in a reverberant concert hall, which produced qualitatively similar results except that thresholds were significantly higher than in the acoustically dry laboratory. 相似文献
10.
A system plus environment conservative model is used to characterize the nonlinear dynamics when the time averaged energy for the system particle starts to decay. The system particle dynamics is regular for low values of the N environment oscillators and becomes chaotic in the interval 13≤N≤15, where the system time averaged energy starts to decay. To characterize the nonlinear motion we estimate the Lyapunov exponent (LE), determine the power spectrum and the Kaplan-Yorke dimension. For much larger values of N the energy of the system particle is completely transferred to the environment and the corresponding LEs decrease. Numerical evidence shows the connection between the variations of the amplitude of the particles energy time oscillation with the time averaged energy decay and trapped trajectories. 相似文献
11.
C Formby L N Morgan T G Forrest J J Raney 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1992,91(1):293-305
The purpose of this study was to compare the role of frequency selectivity in measures of auditory and vibrotactile temporal resolution. In the first experiment, temporal modulation transfer functions for a sinusoidally amplitude modulated (SAM) 250-Hz carrier revealed auditory modulation thresholds significantly lower than corresponding vibrotactile modulation thresholds at SAM frequencies greater than or equal to 100 Hz. In the second experiment, auditory and vibrotactile gap detection thresholds were measured by presenting silent gaps bounded by markers of the same or different frequency. The marker frequency F1 = 250 Hz preceded the silent gap and marker frequencies after the silent gap included F2 = 250, 255, 263, 310, and 325 Hz. Auditory gap detection thresholds were lower than corresponding vibrotactile thresholds for F2 markers less than or equal to 263 Hz, but were greater than the corresponding vibrotactile gap detection thresholds for F2 markers greater than or equal to 310 Hz. When the auditory gap detection thresholds were transformed into filter attenuation values, the results were modeled well by a constant-percentage (10%) bandwidth filter centered on F1. The vibrotactile gap detection thresholds, however, were independent of marker frequency separation. In a third experiment, auditory and vibrotactile rate difference limens (RDLs) were measured for a 250-Hz carrier at SAM rates less than or equal to 100 Hz. Auditory RDLs were lower than corresponding vibrotactile RDLs for standard rates greater than 10 Hz. Combination tones may have confounded auditory performance for standard rates of 80 and 100 Hz. The results from these experiments revealed that frequency selectivity influences auditory measures of temporal resolution, but there was no evidence of frequency selectivity affecting vibrotactile temporal resolution. 相似文献
12.
13.
Oxenham AJ 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》2000,107(4):2215-2223
This study investigated the effect on gap detection of perceptual channels, hypothesized to be tuned to spatial location or fundamental frequency (f0). Thresholds were measured for the detection of a silent temporal gap between two markers. In the first experiment, the markers were broadband noise, presented either binaurally or monaurally. In the binaural conditions, the markers were either diotic, or had a 640-micros interaural time difference (ITD) or a 12-dB interaural level difference (ILD). Reversing the ITD across the two markers had no effect on gap detection relative to the diotic condition. Reversing the ILD across the two markers produced a marked deterioration in performance. However, the same deterioration was observed in the monaural conditions when a 12-dB level difference was introduced between the two markers. The results provide no evidence for the role of spatially tuned neural channels in gap detection. In the second experiment, the markers were harmonic tone complexes, filtered to contain only high, unresolved harmonics. Using complexes with a fixed spectral envelope, where the f0 (of 140 or 350 Hz) was different for the two markers, produced a deterioration in performance, relative to conditions where the f0 remained the same. A larger deterioration was observed when the two markers occupied different spectral regions but had the same f0. This supports the idea that peripheral coding is dominant in determining gap-detection thresholds when the two markers differ along any physical dimension. Higher-order neural coding mechanisms of f0 and spatial location seem to play a smaller role and no role, respectively. 相似文献
14.
合理的推断和预期是加快辨别听觉目标的关键。通过听觉目标辨别的反应时任务,研究了听感知过程中节律性时间期待和空间期待对声音目标辨别的影响。实验中,听觉序列中各声音以规律或不规律的时间节奏、沿有序或随机的空间方位呈现,形成四种期待条件:时间和空间期待兼备、只有时间期待、只有空间期待和没有期待。被试对声音序列末尾随机呈现的听觉目标尽快做按键反应。结果发现:(1)节律性时间期待显著加快被试对目标声音的反应速度;(2)空间期待对目标辨别速度的促进作用仅在时间非规律条件下发生。可见,在辨别听觉目标时,听者优先使用时间期待,并在无有效时间预测信息时使用空间期待,以促成对声音目标的最优知觉加工。 相似文献
15.
16.
Nowadays, it is widely believed that the temporal structure of the auditory nerve fibers' response to sound stimuli plays an important role in auditory perception. An influential hypothesis is that information is extracted from this temporal structure by neural operations akin to an autocorrelation algorithm. The goal of the present work was to test this hypothesis. The stimuli consisted of sequences of unipolar clicks that were high-pass filtered and mixed with low-pass noise so as to exclude spectral cues. In experiment 1, "interfering" clicks were inserted in an otherwise periodic (isochronous) click sequence. Each click belonging to the periodic sequence was followed, after a random portion of the period, by one interfering click. This disrupted the detection of temporal regularity, even when the interfering clicks were 5 dB less intense than the periodic clicks. Experiments 2-4 used click sequences that showed a single peak in their autocorrelation functions. For some sequences, this peak originated from "first-order" temporal regularities, that is from the temporal relations between consecutive clicks. For other sequences, the peak originated instead from "second-order" regularities, relative to nonconsecutive clicks. The detection of second-order regularities appeared to be much more difficult than the detection of comparable first-order regularities. Overall, these results do not tally with the current autocorrelation models of temporal processing. They suggest that the extraction of temporal information from a group of closely spaced spectral components makes no use of time intervals between nonconsecutive peaks of the amplitude envelope. 相似文献
17.
Detection and discrimination of simulated motion of auditory targets in the horizontal plane 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
D W Grantham 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1986,79(6):1939-1949
Three experiments investigated subjects' ability to detect and discriminate the simulated horizontal motion of auditory targets in an anechoic environment. "Moving" stimuli were produced by dynamic application of stereophonic balancing algorithms to a two-loudspeaker system with a 30 degree separation. All stimuli were 500-Hz tones. In experiment 1, subjects had to discriminate a left-to-right moving stimulus from a stationary stimulus pulsed for the same duration (300 or 600 ms). For both durations, minimum audible "movement" angles ("MAMA's") were on the order of 5 degrees for stimuli presented at 0 degrees azimuth (straight ahead), and increased to greater than 30 degrees for stimuli presented at +/- 90 degrees azimuth. Experiment 2 further investigated MAMA's at 0 degrees azimuth, employing two different procedures to track threshold: holding stimulus duration constant (at 100-600 ms) while varying velocity; or holding the velocity constant (at 22 degrees-360 degrees/s) while varying duration. Results from the two procedures agreed with each other and with the MAMA's determined by Perrott and Musicant for actually moving sound sources [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 62, 1463-1466 (1977b)]: As stimulus duration decreased below 100-150 ms, the MAMA's increased sharply from 5 degrees-20 degrees or more, indicating that there is some minimum integration time required for subjects to perform optimally in an auditory spatial resolution task. Experiment 3 determined differential "velocity" thresholds employing simulated reference velocities of 0 degrees-150 degrees/s and stimulus durations of 150-600 ms. As with experiments 1 and 2, the data are more easily summarized by considering angular distance than velocity: For a given "extent of movement" of a reference target, about 4 degrees-10 degrees additional extent is required for threshold discrimination between two "moving" targets, more or less independently of stimulus duration or reference velocity. These data suggest that for the range of simulated velocities employed in these experiments, subjects respond to spatial changes--not velocity per se--when presented with a "motion" detection or discrimination task. 相似文献
18.
E Javel C D Geisler A Ravindran 《The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America》1978,63(4):1093-1104
Responses to two-tone stimuli were recorded from auditory-nerve fibers in anesthetized cats. One tone, the suppressor, was set at a frequency above characteristic frequency and was fixed in intensity. A second tone was set at an excitatory frequency and was varied in intensity. The suppressor tone, when set at a sufficient level, always reduced the response to the excitatory tone by an amount equivalent to a fixed number of decibels, regardless of the excitatory tone's intensity. Estimates of suppression magnitude were derived from shifts in rate-intensity function obtained when the suppressor tone was present relative to the functions obtained for the excitatory tone alone. When suppressor-tone intensity was increased, suppression magnitude likewise increased. When the two tones were increasingly separated in frequency, either by varying the excitor or by varying the suppressor, suppression magnitude decreased monotonically. Suppression behaved in the same manner regardless of whether suppresor tone was excitatory or nonexcitatory. When frequency separation was small enough and when both tones were above the neuron's characteristic frequency, responses synchronized to low-order combination tones could be elicited. These responses usually possessed different rate-intensity characteristics and resulted in estimates of suppression magnitude which were spuriously low. When frequency separation is normalized with regard to position of traveling wave maxima within the cochlear duct, the magnitude of two-tone suppression for a given suppressor-tone intensity is seen to be frequency independent. 相似文献
19.
The ability of listeners to detect a temporal gap in a 1600-Hz-wide noiseband (target) was studied as a function of the absence and presence of concurrent stimulation by a second 1600-Hz-wide noiseband (distractor) with a nonoverlapping spectrum. Gap detection thresholds for single noisebands centered on 1.0, 2.0, 4.0, and 5.0 kHz were in the range from 4 to 6 ms, and were comparable to those described in previous studies. Gap thresholds for the same target noisebands were only modestly improved by the presence of a synchronously gated gap in a second frequency band. Gap thresholds were unaffected by the presence of a continuous distractor that was either proximate or remote from the target frequency band. Gap thresholds for the target noiseband were elevated if the distractor noiseband also contained a gap which "roved" in time in temporal proximity to the target gap. This effect was most marked in inexperienced listeners. Between-channel gap thresholds, obtained using leading and trailing markers that differed in frequency, were high in all listeners, again consistent with previous findings. The data are discussed in terms of the levels of the auditory perceptual processing stream at which the listener can voluntarily access auditory events in distinct frequency channels. 相似文献
20.
《Physica B+C》1977
Measurements of the magnetization M and dM/dt have been made as a function of magnetic field and temperature in materials based on the composition DyCo2. The results are compared with the tunnelling model of Egami and a simple thermal excitation model. 相似文献