首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
2.
This study investigates the controversy regarding the influence of age on the acoustic reflex threshold for broadband noise, 500-, 1000-, 2000-, and 4000-Hz activators between Jerger et al. [Mono. Contemp. Audiol. 1 (1978)] and Jerger [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 66 (1979)] on the one hand and Silman [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 66 (1979)] and others on the other. The acoustic reflex thresholds for broadband noise, 500-, 1000-, 2000-, and 4000-Hz activators were evaluated under two measurement conditions. Seventy-two normal-hearing ears were drawn from 72 subjects ranging in age from 20-69 years. The results revealed that age was correlated with the acoustic reflex threshold for BBN activator but not for any of the tonal activators; the correlation was stronger under the 1-dB than under the 5-dB measurement condition. Also, the mean acoustic reflex thresholds for broadband noise activator were essentially similar to those reported by Jerger et al. (1978) but differed from those obtained in this study under the 1-dB measurement condition.  相似文献   

3.
Recent studies of the relation between loudness and intensity difference limens (DLs) suggest that, if two tones of the same frequency are equally loud, they will have equal relative DLs [R. S. Schlauch and C.C. Wier, J. Speech Hear. Res. 30, 13-20 (1987); J.J. Zwislocki and H.N. Jordan, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 79, 772-780 (1986)]. To test this hypothesis, loudness matches and intensity DLs for a 1000-Hz pure tone in quiet and in a 40-dB SPL spectrum level broadband noise were obtained for four subjects with normal hearing. The DLs were obtained in both gated- and continuous-pedestal conditions. Contrary to previous reports, equally loud tones do not yield equal relative DLs at several midintensities in the gated condition and at many intensities in the continuous condition. While the equal-loudness, equal-relative-DL hypothesis is not supported by the data, the relation between loudness and intensity discrimination appears to be well described by a model reported by Houtsma et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 68, 807-813 (1980)].  相似文献   

4.
The acoustic behavior in thermo-viscous gas mixtures, both in proximity of walls and far from them (outside the boundary layers), involves deviations from the adiabatic and laminar movements in pure gases, which result from the influence of several diffusive fields, namely, shear, entropic, and concentration variation fields (their energy being provided by the acoustic field itself). Owing to the boundary conditions, that are slip condition, isothermal condition and concentration flux vanishing on the walls, a strong coupling between these fields occurs inside the boundary layers while their effects appear to be simple additive processes in the bulk of the medium. Although recent literature on this subject leads to interesting results, opening the way to several new issues [R. Raspet et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105, 65-73 (1999); R. Raspet et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 112, 1414-1422 (2002); G. W. Swift and P. S. Spoor, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 106, 1794-1800 (1999); D. A. Geller and G. W. Swift, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 111, 1675-1684 (2002)], the results available still have limitations because they do not provide complete solutions for the propagative and diffusive fields throughout and beyond the boundary layers. The present work aims at providing these solutions in the whole domains considered. The results allow interpreting analytically the behavior of the fields above mentioned in closed cavities and ducts, and particularly in spherical cavities which are best suited to develop metrological applications.  相似文献   

5.
Previous studies indicate that monkey pure tone frequency discrimination is quantitatively and qualitatively very different from that of humans: Monkey DLs at 1.0 and 2.0 kHz are up to 20 times larger than human DLs, and monkeys DLs increase as sensation level increases, in contrast to human DLs [Sinnott et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 78, 1977-1985 (1985); Sinnott et al., J. Comp. Psychol. 101, 126-131 (1987)]. These results led to an hypothesis that monkey frequency discrimination is more dependent upon "rate" coding than is that of humans. The present study compared monkey and human DLs for formant frequency changes along three synthetic vowel continua /I-i/, /ae-epsilon/, and /a-v/. Here, monkey DLs for formants near 1.0 and 2.0 kHz (32-48 Hz) were only about two to three times larger than human DLs (11-21 Hz), and both monkeys and humans exhibited relatively similar, flat sensation level functions. Taken together, these data indicate that monkey and human frequency discrimination is more similar in the case of a complex vowel stimulus than in the case of a simple pure tone stimulus. Results are discussed in relation to "rate" versus "temporal" coding of tones and vowels in the auditory system.  相似文献   

6.
Cochlear model calculations are shown to be in reasonable agreement with recent low-frequency measurements of intracochlear pressures and the cochlear input impedance of the cat [V. Nedzelnitsky, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 68, 1676-1689 (1980); T. J. Lynch, III, V. Nedzelnitsky, and W. T. Peake, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 72, 108-130 (1982)]. Included in the cochlear model are perilymph viscosity, the measured variation of the area of the scala vestibuli with distance from the stapes [P. Dallos, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 48, 489-499 (1970)], and finite impedance of the round window membrane. The WKB approximation and its extension to the low-frequency region is used in order to exhibit explicitly the dependence of the model results on the cochlear parameters.  相似文献   

7.
Koizumi et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 82, 1179-1192 (1987)] have proposed a way to incorporate mucosal waves into previous two-mass mechanical models of the vocal folds. This was accomplished by allowing the mass of the masses to vary with time. The equations of motion Koizumi et al. used to mathematically describe this model neglected terms from the time rate of change of momentum of Newton's second law. In this letter, approximations of the magnitude of this term indicate that it must not be neglected.  相似文献   

8.
9.
Listeners' ability to understand speech in adverse listening conditions is partially due to the redundant nature of speech. Natural redundancies are often lost or altered when speech is filtered, such as done in AI/SII experiments. It is important to study how listeners recognize speech when the speech signal is unfiltered and the entire broadband spectrum is present. A correlational method [R. A. Lutfi, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 97, 1333-1334 (1995); V. M. Richards and S. Zhu, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 95, 423-424 (1994)] has been used to determine how listeners use spectral cues to perceive nonsense syllables when the full speech spectrum is present [K. A. Doherty and C. W. Turner, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 3769-3773 (1996); C. W. Turner et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 104, 1580-1585 (1998)]. The experiments in this study measured spectral-weighting strategies for more naturally occurring speech stimuli, specifically sentences, using a correlational method for normal-hearing listeners. Results indicate that listeners placed the greatest weight on spectral information within bands 2 and 5 (562-1113 and 2807-11,000 Hz), respectively. Spectral-weighting strategies for sentences were also compared to weighting strategies for nonsense syllables measured in a previous study (C. W. Turner et al., 1998). Spectral-weighting strategies for sentences were different from those reported for nonsense syllables.  相似文献   

10.
This paper proposes a modified boundary condition to improve the room-acoustic prediction accuracy of a diffusion equation model. Previous boundary conditions for the diffusion equation model have certain limitations which restrict its application to a certain number of room types. The boundary condition employing the Sabine absorption coefficient [V. Valeau et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 119, 1504-1513 (2006)] cannot predict the sound field well when the absorption coefficient is high, while the boundary condition employing the Eyring absorption coefficient [Y. Jing and N. Xiang, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 121, 3284-3287 (2007); A. Billon et al., Appl. Acoust. 69, (2008)] has a singularity whenever any surface material has an absorption coefficient of 1.0. The modified boundary condition is derived based on an analogy between sound propagation and light propagation. Simulated and experimental data are compared to verify the modified boundary condition in terms of room-acoustic parameter prediction. The results of this comparison suggest that the modified boundary condition is valid for a range of absorption coefficient values and successfully eliminates the singularity problem.  相似文献   

11.
Reflection of retrograde waves within the cochlea and at the stapes   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
A number of authors [de Boer and Viergever, Hear. Res. 13, 101-112 (1984); de Boer et al., in Peripheral Auditory Mechanisms (Springer-Verlag, Berlin, 1986); Hear. Res. 23, 1-7 (1986); Viergever, in Auditory Frequency Selectivity (Plenum, New York, 1986), pp. 31-38; Kaernbach et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 81, 408-411 (1987)] have argued that backward-traveling waves, in striking contrast to waves traveling forward towards the helicotrema, suffer appreciable reflection as they move through the basal turns of the cochlea. Such reflection, if present, would have important consequences for understanding the nature and strength of otoacoustic emissions. The apparent asymmetry in reflection of cochlear waves is shown, however, to be an artifact of the boundary condition those authors impose at the stapes: conventional cochlear models are found not to generate reflections of waves traveling in either direction even when the wavelength changes rapidly and the WKB approximation breaks down. Although backward-traveling waves are not reflected by the secular variation of the geometrical and mechanical characteristics of the cochlea, they are reflected when they reach the stapes. The magnitude of that boundary reflection is computed for the cat and shown to be a large, rapidly varying function of frequency.  相似文献   

12.
Time domain cochlear models have primarily followed a method introduced by Allen and Sondhi [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 66, 123-132 (1979)]. Recently the "state space formalism" proposed by Elliott et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 122, 2759-2771 (2007)] has been used to simulate a wide range of nonlinear cochlear models. It used a one-dimensional approach that is extended to two dimensions in this paper, using the finite element method. The recently developed "state space formalism" in fact shares a close relationship to the earlier approach. Working from Diependaal et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 82, 1655-1666 (1987)] the two approaches are compared and the relationship formalized. Understanding this relationship allows models to be converted from one to the other in order to utilize each of their strengths. A second method to derive the state space matrices required for the "state space formalism" is also presented. This method offers improved numerical properties because it uses the information available about the model more effectively. Numerical results support the claims regarding fluid dimension and the underlying similarity of the two approaches. Finally, the recent advances in the state space formalism [Bertaccini and Sisto, J. Comp. Phys. 230, 2575-2587 (2011)] are discussed in terms of this relationship.  相似文献   

13.
For 23 cadaver ears from Norwegian cattle, frequency characteristics for the round-window volume displacement relative to the sound pressure at the eardrum have been measured, and are compared to earlier results for human ears [M. Kringlebotn and T. Gundersen, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 77(1), 159-164 (1985)]. For human as well as for cattle ears, mean amplitude curves have peaks at about 0.7 kHz. At lower frequencies, the mean amplitude for cattle ears is about 5 dB smaller than for human ears. The amplitude curves cross at about 2 kHz, and toward higher frequencies the amplitude for cattle ears becomes increasingly larger. If amplitude curves are roughly approximated by straight lines above 1 kHz, the slope for cattle ears is about -5 dB/octave as compared to about -15 dB/octave for human ears. The phase of the round-window volume displacement lags behind the phase of the sound pressure at the tympanic membrane. The phase lag is close to zero below 0.2 kHz, but increases to about 3.5 pi at 20 kHz for cattle ears, as compared to less than 2 pi for human ears. Further investigations are needed in order to explain the observed differences. Sound transmission in the ear decreases with an increasing static pressure difference across the tympanic membrane, especially at frequencies below 1 kHz, where pressure differences of 10 and 60 cm water cause mean transmission losses of about 10 and 26 dB, respectively, the losses being somewhat larger for overpressures than for underpressures in the ear canal. At higher frequencies, the transmission losses are smaller. For small overpressures, and in a limited frequency range near 3 kHz, even some transmission enhancement may occur. Static pressure variations in the inner ear have only a minor influence on sound transmission. Static pressures relative to the middle ear in the range 0-60 cm water cause mean sound transmission losses less than 5 dB below 1 kHz, and negligible losses at higher frequencies.  相似文献   

14.
Models of the additivity of masking   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   

15.
Because they consist, in large part, of random turbulent noise, fricatives present a challenge to attempts to specify the phonetic correlates of phonological features. Previous research has focused on temporal properties, acoustic power, and a variety of spectral properties of fricatives in a number of contexts [Jongman et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108, 1252-1263 (2000); Jesus and Shadle, J. Phonet. 30, 437-467 (2002); Crystal and House, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 1553-1573 (1988a)]. However, no systematic investigation of the effects of focus and prosodic context on fricative production has been carried out. Manipulation of explicit focus can serve to selectively exaggerate linguistically relevant properties of speech in much the same manner as stress [de Jong, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 97, 491-504 (1995); de Jong, J. Phonet. 32, 493-516 (2004); de Jong and Zawaydeh, J. Phonet. 30, 53-75 (2002)]. This experimental technique was exploited to investigate acoustic power along with temporal and spectral characteristics of American English fricatives in two prosodic contexts, to probe whether native speakers selectively attend to subsegmental features, and to consider variability in fricative production across speakers. While focus in general increased noise power and duration, speakers did not selectively enhance spectral features of the target fricatives.  相似文献   

16.
A series of laboratory experiments was conducted to obtain high-quality data for acoustic propagation in shallow water waveguides with sloping elastic bottoms. Accurate modeling of transmission loss in these waveguides can be performed with the variable rotated parabolic equation method. Results from an earlier experiment with a flat or sloped slab of polyvinyl chloride (PVC) demonstrated the necessity of accounting for elasticity in the bottom and the ability of the model to produce benchmark-quality agreement with experimental data [J. M. Collis et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 122, 1987-1993 (2007)]. This paper presents results of a second experiment, using two PVC slabs joined at an angle to create a waveguide with variable bottom slope. Acoustic transmissions over the 100-300 kHz band were received on synthetic horizontal arrays for two source positions. The PVC slabs were oriented to produce three different simulated waveguides: flat bottom followed by downslope, upslope followed by flat bottom, and upslope followed by downslope. Parabolic equation solutions for treating variable slopes are benchmarked against the data.  相似文献   

17.
The underwater hearing sensitivity of a young male harbor porpoise for tonal signals of various signal durations was quantified by using a behavioral psychophysical technique. The animal was trained to respond only when it detected an acoustic signal. Fifty percent detection thresholds were obtained for tonal signals (15 frequencies between 0.25-160 kHz, durations 0.5-5000 ms depending on the frequency; 134 frequency-duration combinations in total). Detection thresholds were quantified by varying signal amplitude by the 1-up 1-down staircase method. The hearing thresholds increased when the signal duration fell below the time constant of integration. The time constants, derived from an exponential model of integration [Plomp and Bouman, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 31, 749-758 (1959)], varied from 629 ms at 2 kHz to 39 ms at 64 kHz. The integration times of the porpoises were similar to those of other mammals including humans, even though the porpoise is a marine mammal and a hearing specialist. The results enable more accurate estimations of the distances at which porpoises can detect short-duration environmental tonal signals. The audiogram thresholds presented by Kastelein et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 112, 334-344 (2002)], after correction for the frequency bandwidth of the FM signals, are similar to the results of the present study for signals of 1500 ms duration. Harbor porpoise hearing is more sensitive between 2 and 10 kHz, and less sensitive above 10 kHz, than formerly believed.  相似文献   

18.
This letter concerns the paper "Intraspecific and geographic variation of West Indian manatee (Trichechus manatus spp.) vocalizations" [Nowacek et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 114, 66-69 (2003)]. The purpose here is to correct the fundamental frequency range and information on intraindividual variation in the vocalizations of Amazonian manatees reported by Nowacek et al. (2003) in citing the paper "Signature information and individual recognition in the isolation calls of Amazonian manatees, Trichechus inunguis (Mammalia: Sirenia)" [Sousa-Lima et al., Anim. Behav. 63, 301-310 (2002)].  相似文献   

19.
Recently, Eddins and Barber [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103, 2578-2589 (1998)] and Hall et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103, 2573-2577 (1998)] independently reported that greater masking of interaurally phase-reversed (S pi) tones was produced by diotic low-noise noise than by diotic Gaussian noise. Based on quantitative analyses, Eddins and Barber suggested that their results could not be accounted for by assuming that listeners' judgments were based on constant-criterion changes in the normalized interaural correlation produced by adding the S pi signal to the diotic masker. In particular, they showed that a model like the one previously employed by Bernstein and Trahiotis [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 3774-3784 (1996)] predicted an ordering of thresholds between the conditions of interest that was opposite to that observed. Bernstein and Trahiotis computed the normalized interaural correlation subsequent to half-wave, square-law rectification and low-pass filtering, the parameters of which were chosen to mimic peripheral auditory processing. In this report, it is demonstrated that augmenting the model by adding a physiologically valid stage of "envelope compression" prior to rectification and low-pass filtering provides a remedy. The new model not only accounts for the data obtained by Eddins and Barber (and the similar data obtained by Hall et al.), but also does not diminish the highly successful account of the comprehensive set of data that gave rise to the original form of the model. Therefore, models based on the computation of the normalized interaural correlation appear to remain valid because they can account, both quantitatively and qualitatively, for a wide variety of binaural detection and discrimination data.  相似文献   

20.
Psychophysical, basilar-membrane (BM), and single nerve-fiber tuning curves, as well as suppression of distortion-product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs), all give rise to frequency tuning patterns with stereotypical features. Similarities and differences between the behaviors of these tuning functions, both in normal conditions and following various cochlear insults, have been documented. While neural tuning curves (NTCs) and BM tuning curves behave similarly both before and after cochlear insults known to disrupt frequency selectivity, DPOAE suppression tuning curves (STCs) do not necessarily mirror these responses following either administration of ototoxins [Martin et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 104, 972-983 (1998)] or exposure to temporarily damaging noise [Howard et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 111, 285-296 (2002)]. However, changes in STC parameters may be predictive of other changes in cochlear function such as cochlear immaturity in neonatal humans [Abdala, Hear. Res. 121, 125-138 (1998)]. To determine the effects of noise-induced permanent auditory dysfunction on STC parameters, rabbits were exposed to high-level noise that led to permanent reductions in DPOAE level, and comparisons between pre- and postexposure DPOAE levels and STCs were made. Statistical comparisons of pre- and postexposure STC values at CF revealed consistent basal shifts in the frequency region of greatest cochlear damage, whereas thresholds, Q10dB, and tip-to-tail gain values were not reliably altered. Additionally, a large percentage of high-frequency lobes associated with third tone interference phenomena, that were exhibited in some data sets, were dramatically reduced following noise exposure. Thus, previously described areas of DPOAE interference above f2 may also be studied using this type of experimental manipulation [Martin et al., Hear. Res. 136, 105-123 (1999); Mills, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107, 2586-2602 (2002)].  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号