首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
Additivity of simultaneous masking   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Simultaneous masking functions (signal level at threshold versus masker level) were obtained for equally intense maskers presented individually and in pairs. The signal was a 2.0-kHz sinusoid. The pairs of maskers were (1) two sinusoids with frequencies 1.9 and 2.1 kHz, (2) two narrow bands of noise (50 Hz wide) centered at 1.9 and 2.1 kHz, (3) two narrow bands of noise (50 Hz wide) centered at 1.8 and 1.9 kHz, and (4) the 1.9-kHz sinusoid combined with the narrow band of noise centered at 2.1 kHz. The pairs of maskers produced anywhere from 10 to 17 dB of masking beyond that predicted from the simple sum of the masking produced by the individual maskers. The amount of this "additional masking" was independent of masker level. Adding a continuous low level background noise reduced the amount of additional masking only slightly (approximately 5 dB). The data are consistent with a model in which the effects of the maskers are summed after undergoing independent compressive transformations.  相似文献   

3.
Lutfi [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 73, 262-267 (1983)] compared simultaneous masking functions (signal threshold versus masker level) for individual sinusoidal and narrow-band noise maskers, and for those maskers presented in pairs. Lutfi found that the pairs of maskers produced 10-17 dB "excess" masking over that predicted from the linear sum of their individual masking and explained the results in terms of a model in which the effects of the maskers are summed after undergoing independent compressive transformations. This paper describes experiments similar to those of Lutfi, and presents evidence suggesting that Lutfi's results may have been influenced by two factors: (1) combination-product detection, and (2) the use of different detection cues for single maskers and for pairs of maskers. Experiment I showed that when the stimulus conditions were chosen so as to minimize the likelihood of combination-product detection, "excess" masking was only 3-5 dB. Experiment II supported the idea that for a single narrow-band noise masker, subjects make use of the relatively slow envelope fluctuations to enhance performance. When two independent narrow-band noise maskers are added, the effectiveness of this cue is reduced, and between 3 and 9 dB of "excess" masking occurs. When the two noises are derived from the same source, and have correlated envelope fluctuations, no "excess" masking occurs. The results indicate that Lufti's compressive-nonlinearity model clearly fails in some situations.  相似文献   

4.
The additivity of nonsimultaneous masking was studied using Gaussian-shaped tone pulses (referred to as Gaussians) as masker and target stimuli. Combinations of up to four temporally separated Gaussian maskers with an equivalent rectangular bandwidth of 600 Hz and an equivalent rectangular duration of 1.7 ms were tested. Each masker was level-adjusted to produce approximately 8 dB of masking. Excess masking (exceeding linear additivity) was generally stronger than reported in the literature for longer maskers and comparable target levels. A model incorporating a compressive input/output function, followed by a linear summation stage, underestimated excess masking when using an input/output function derived from literature data for longer maskers and comparable target levels. The data could be predicted with a more compressive input/output function. Stronger compression may be explained by assuming that the Gaussian stimuli were too short to evoke the medial olivocochlear reflex (MOCR), whereas for longer maskers tested previously the MOCR caused reduced compression. Overall, the interpretation of the data suggests strong basilar membrane compression for very short stimuli.  相似文献   

5.
6.
7.
The overshoot effect can be reduced by temporary hearing loss induced by aspirin or exposure to intense sound. The present study simulated a hearing loss at 4.0 kHz via pure-tone forward masking and examined the effect of the simulation on threshold for a 10-ms, 4.0-kHz signal presented 1 ms after the onset of a 400-ms, broadband noise masker whose spectrum level was 20 dB SPL. Masker frequency was 3.6, 4.0, or 4.2 kHz, and masker level was 80 dB SPL. Subject-dependent delays were determined such that 10 or 20 dB of masking at 4.0 kHz was produced. In general, the pure-tone forward masker did not reduce the simultaneous-masked threshold, suggesting that elevating threshold with a pure-tone forward masker does not sufficiently simulate the effect of a temporary hearing loss on overshoot.  相似文献   

8.
The differences in the suppression effect observed in forward and backward masking are consistent with an interpretation that suppression in forward masking results from a reduction of the effective level of the masker in the aditory periphery, and that the suppression in backward masking is influenced by these peripheral processes, but is dominated by additional, central processes. This conclusion is supported by experiments that show differences in the effect of ipsilateral versus contralateral presentation of the suppressor, and differences in the amount of the suppression observed as a function of the level, duration, and frequency of the suppressor.  相似文献   

9.
In forward masking, performance may be affected by confusion, that is, by the difficulty of discriminating a suprathreshold signal from the preceding masker. This study investigated confusion effects for forward maskers composed of repeated bursts of a 100-Hz sinusoid followed by sinusoidal signals; such "pulsing" maskers produce confusion when the properties of the signal are identical to those of an individual masker "pulse." The level, frequency, and duration of the signal relative to an individual masker pulse, as well as offset-onset delay, were varied to determine the minimum change necessary to eliminate confusion. For maskers composed of 20-ms pulses, confusion was eliminated by changes in signal level of 5 dB or changes in signal frequency of 30 to 40 Hz. For maskers composed of 10-, 20-, or 40-ms pulses, confusion was eliminated by signal delays of 8 to 16 ms or by signal durations less than half or greater than twice the masker-pulse duration. Results with adaptive procedures designed to measure confusion-free or confusion-determined thresholds suggest that confusion effects can be minimized or avoided by extensive listener training with a procedure in which the signal and masker are not presented at similar intensities.  相似文献   

10.
Frequency difference limens for pure tones preceded by a forward masker or followed by a backward masker were obtained across a wide range of signal levels. Relkin and Doucet [Hear. Res. 55, 215-222 (1991)] have shown that at a masker-signal delay of 100 ms, the thresholds of high-SR (spontaneous rate) auditory-nerve fibers are recovered, while the low-SR fiber thresholds are not. Therefore, forward-masked frequency discrimination potentially offers a method to investigate the role of low-SR fibers in the coding of frequency. It has been shown that when an intense forward masker is presented 100 ms before a pure-tone signal, intensity difference limens are elevated for mid-level signals [Zeng et al., Hear. Res. 55, 223-230 (1991)]. However, Plack and Viemeister [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 92, 3097-3101 (1992)] have shown that a similar elevation in the intensity difference limen is obtained under conditions of backward masking, where selective adaptation of the auditory neurons would not be expected to occur. A condition of backward-masked frequency discrimination was therefore included to investigate the role of interference resulting from adding additional stimuli to a discrimination task. For signals at 1000 and 6000 Hz, there was no effect of a forward masker upon frequency difference limens. For the backward-masked conditions, an elevation of the frequency difference limen was observed at all signal levels, demonstrating that the effects of forward and backward maskers upon frequency discrimination are dissimilar and suggesting that cognitive effects are present in backward-masked discrimination tasks.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

11.
Threshold shifts for the detection of vibrotactile test stimuli were determined as a function of the intensity of a masker. A 50-ms sinusoidal test stimulus was applied to the thenar eminence of the hand 25 ms after the termination of a 700-ms sinusoidal masker applied to the same site. The intensity of the masker was varied over a range of 0-44 dB SL. The frequency of the masker was either 15 or 250 Hz and the frequency of the test stimulus was either 15, 25, 100, or 250 Hz. The results support the hypothesis that the detection of vibrotactile stimuli is mediated by at least two receptor systems which do not mask each other.  相似文献   

12.
13.
Thresholds for the detection of a 50-ms test stimulus delivered to the thenar eminence were measured as a function of the time interval between the offset of a 500-ms masking stimulus and the onset of the test stimulus (delta t). The frequency of the masker and the test stimulus was the same during a particular testing session and was either 25 or 250 Hz. At all values of delta t, older subjects exhibited significantly more masking than did young subjects. The effects of age were greater for stimuli that primarily affect the Pacinian system (250 Hz) than those that primarily affect non-Pacinian systems (25 Hz). Psychophysical measurements of the apparent duration of tactile sensations suggest that both sensory persistence and adaptation are affected by aging. Since adaptation seemed to be the more dominant factor for stimuli with durations as long as 500 ms, it was concluded that the effects of aging on forward masking seen in our study were due mainly to increased amounts of adaptation produced by the masker.  相似文献   

14.
Experiments on intensity discrimination determine the size of the smallest detectable increment added to a fixed pedestal. This paper examines the effects of a masker which either precedes the pedestal (forward masking) or is simultaneous with the pedestal. The increment and pedestal were 1-kHz tones masked in forward masking by pure tones and in simultaneous masking by a broadband noise. Simultaneous masking by the broadband noise eliminates the "near miss" to Weber's law, and thus degrades intensity discrimination at high pedestal levels. Forward masking by the pure tone also degrades intensity discrimination, which may, in part, be explained by the elimination of the near miss. However, the effect on intensity discrimination in some cases is greater in forward than in simultaneous masking, suggesting that some additional process (e.g., adaptation) is involved.  相似文献   

15.
Forward masking, as measured behaviorally, is defined as an increase in a signal's detection threshold resulting from a preceding masker. Previously, forward masking in the auditory nerve has been measured as a reduction in the neural response to a signal when preceded by a masker. However, detection threshold depends on both the magnitude of the response to the signal and the variance of the response. Thus changes in detectability cannot be inferred from response reduction alone. Relkin and Pelli (1987) have described a two-interval forced-choice procedure that may be used to measure the threshold for the detection of a probe signal in recordings of spike counts in single auditory neurons. These methods have been used to study the forward masking of characteristic frequency probe tones by characteristic frequency maskers as masker intensity was varied. Although the masker does reduce the detectability of the probe tone, it was found that the threshold shifts are much less than those observed behaviorally, particularly for intense maskers. In part, the small threshold shifts can be attributed to the reduction in response variance following the masker, which is the result of the adaptation of spontaneous activity. These results imply that behavioral forward masking must result from suboptimal processing of spike counts from auditory neurons at a location central to the auditory nerve.  相似文献   

16.
Psychophysical tuning curves measured in simultaneous and forward masking   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
The level of a masker necessary to mask a probe fixed in frequency and level was determined as a function of masker frequency using a two-interval forced-choice technique. Both simultaneous- and forward- masking techniques were used. Parameters investigated include the level of the probe tone and the frequency of the probe tone. The general form of the psychophysical tuning curves obtained in this way is quite similar to that of single-neurone tuning curves, when low-level probe tones are used. However, the curves obtained to forward masking generally show sharper tips and steeper slopes than those found in simultaneous masking, and they are also generally sharper than neurophysiological tuning curves. For frequencies of the masker close to that of the probe a simultaneous masker was sometimes less effective than a forward masker. The results are discussed in relation to possible lateral suppression effects in simultaneous masking, and in relation to the observer's use of pitch cues in forward masking. It is concluded that neither the simultaneous-masking curves nor the forward-masking curves are likely to give an accurate representation of human neural tuning curves.  相似文献   

17.
We compare psychophysical tuning curves obtained with sinusoidal and narrow-band (50-Hz wide) noise maskers in both simultaneous and forward masking. In one experiment, we examine the effects of different combinations of duration and intensity of the 1-kHz sinusoidal signal. In a second experiment, we compare tuning curves obtained with a sinusoidal signal to those obtained with a noise signal. In both experiments, a narrow-band noise is a more effective simultaneous masker than a sinusoid for masker frequencies near the signal frequency. We argue that this is probably due to the use of different detection cues in the presence of sinusoidal and noise maskers, and that the greater masking produced by the noise is not simply due to its greater variability. As observed in other studies, tuning curves are narrower in forward masking than in simultaneous masking.  相似文献   

18.
This study examined the effect of an on-frequency precursor on growth-of-masking (GOM) functions measured using an off-frequency masker. The signal was a 6-ms, 4-kHz tone. A GOM function was measured using a 40-ms, 2.8-kHz tone (the off-frequency masker). GOM functions were then measured with an on-frequency, fixed level precursor presented before the off-frequency masker. The precursor was 50 or 60 dB SPL, and 160 ms in duration. For the 60-dB SPL precursor, a 40-ms duration was also used. Two-line functions were fit to the GOM data to estimate the basilar membrane input-output function. The precursors reduced the gain of the input-output function, and this decrease was graded with precursor level. Both precursor durations had the same effect on gain. Changes in masking following a precursor were larger than would be predicted by additivity of masking. The observed decrease in gain may be consistent with activation of the medial olivocochlear reflex by the precursor.  相似文献   

19.
Psychophysical pulse-train forward-masking (PTFM) recovery functions were measured in fifteen subjects with the Nucleus mini-22 cochlear implant and six subjects with the Clarion cochlear implant. Masker and probe stimuli were 500-Hz trains of 200- or 77-micros/phase biphasic current pulses. Electrode configurations were bipolar for Nucleus subjects and monopolar for Clarion subjects. Masker duration was 320 ms. Probe duration was either 10 ms or 30 ms. Recovery functions were measured for a high-level masker on a middle electrode in all 21 subjects, on apical and basal electrodes in 7 of the Nucleus and 3 of the Clarion subjects, and for multiple masker levels on the middle electrode in 8 Nucleus subjects and 6 Clarion subjects. Recovery functions were described by an exponential process in which threshold shift (in microA) decreased exponentially with increasing time delay between the offset of the masker pulse train and the offset of the probe pulse train. All but 3 of the 21 subjects demonstrated recovery time constants on a middle electrode that were less than 95 ms. The mean time constant for these 18 subjects was 54 ms (s.d. 17 ms). Three other subjects tested on three electrodes exhibited time constants larger than 95 ms from an apical electrode only. Growth-of-masking slopes depended upon time delay, as expected from an exponential recovery process, i.e., progressively shallower slopes were observed at time delays of 10 ms and 50 ms. Recovery of threshold shift (in microA) for PTFM in electrical hearing behaves inthe same way as recovery of threshold shift (in dB) for pure-tone forward masking in acoustic hearing. This supports the concept that linear microamps are the electrical equivalent of acoustic decibels. Recovery from PTFM was not related to speech recognition in a simple manner. Three subjects with prolonged PTFM recovery demonstrated poor speech scores. The remaining subjects with apparently normal PTFM recovery demonstrated speech scores ranging from poor to excellent. Findings suggest that normal PTFM recovery is only one of several factors associated with good speech recognition in cochlear-implant listeners. Comparisons of recovery curves for 10- and 30-ms probe durations in two subjects showed little or no temporal integration at time delays less than 95 ms where recovery functions have steep slopes. The same subjects exhibited large amounts of temporal integration at longer time delays where recovery slopes are more gradual. This suggests that probe detection depends primarily on detection of the final pulses in the probe stimulus and supports the use of offset-to-offset time delays for characterizing PTFM recovery in electric hearing.  相似文献   

20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号