首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
We propose to use didactical theory for the design of educational software. Here we present a set of didactical conditions, and explain how they shape the software design of Cabri-Euclide, a microworld used to learn “mathematical proof ” in a geometry setting. The aim is to design software that does not include a predefined knowledge of problem solution. Key features of the system are its ability to verify local coherence, and not to apply any global and automatic deduction.  相似文献   

2.
In undergraduate mathematics courses, pre-service elementary school teachers are often faced with the task of re-learning some of the concepts they themselves struggled with in their own schooling. This often involves different cognitive processes and psychological issues than initial learning: pre-service teachers have had many more opportunities to construct understandings and representations than initial learners, some of which may be more complex and engrained; pre-service teachers are likely to have created deeply-held–and often negative–beliefs and attitudes toward certain mathematical ideas and processes. In our recent research, we found that pre-service teachers who used a particular computer-based microworld, one emphasising visual representations of and experimental interactions with elementary number theory concepts, overcame many cognitive and psychological difficulties reported in the literature. In this study, we investigate the possibilities of using a similarly-designed microworld that involves a set of rational number concepts. We describe the affordances of this microworld, both in terms of pre-service teacher learning and research on pre-service teacher learning, namely, the helpful “window” it gave us on the mathematical meaning-making of pre-service teachers. We also show how their interactions with this microworld provided many with a new and aesthetically-rich set of visualisations and experiences.  相似文献   

3.
A case study of two 5th-Grade children, Jason and Laura, is presented who participated in the teaching experiment, Children’s Construction of the Rational Numbers of Arithmetic. The case study begins on the 29th of November of their 5th-Grade in school and ends on the 5th of April of the same school year. Two basic problems were of interest in the case study. The first was to provide an analysis of the concepts and operations that are involved in the construction of three fractional schemes: a commensurate fractional scheme, a fractional composition scheme, and a fractional adding scheme. The second was to provide an analysis of the contribution of interactive mathematical activity in the construction of these schemes. The phrase, “commensurate factional scheme” refers to the concepts and operations that are involved in transforming a given fraction into another fraction that are both measures of an identical quantity. Likewise, “fractional composition scheme” refers to the concepts and operations that are involved in finding how much, say, 1/3 of 1/4 of a quantity is of the whole quantity, and “fractional adding scheme” refers to the concepts and operations involved in finding how much, say, 1/3 of a quantity joined to 1/4 of a quantity is of the whole quantity. Critical protocols were abstracted from the teaching episodes with the two children that illustrate what is meant by the schemes, changes in the children’s concepts and operations, and the interactive mathematical activity that was involved. The body of the case study consists of an on-going analysis of the children’s interactive mathematical activity and changes in that activity. The last section of the case study consists of an analysis of the constitutive aspects of the children’s constructive activity, including the role of social interaction and nonverbal interactions of the children with each other and with the computer software we used in teaching the children.  相似文献   

4.
This article describes how children use an expressive microworld to articulate ideas about how to make a game seem fair with the use of randomness. Our aim in this study is to disentangle different flavours of fairness and to find out how children used each flavour to make sense of potentially complex behaviour. In order to achieve this, a spatial computer game was designed to enable children to examine the consequences of their attempts to make the game fair. The study investigates how 23 children, aged between 5.5 and 8 years, engaged in constructing a crucial part of a mechanism for a fair spatial lottery machine (microworld). In particular, the children tried to construct a fair game given a situation in which the key elements happened randomly. The children could select objects, determine their properties, and arrange their spatial layout in the machine. The study is based on task-based interviewing of children who were interacting with the computer game. The study shows that children have various cognitive resources for constructing a random fair environment. The spatial arrangement, the visualisation and the manipulations in the lottery machine allow us gain a view into the children’s thinking of the two central concepts, fairness and randomness. The paper reports on two main strategies by which the children attempted to achieve a balance in the lottery machine. One involves arranging the balls symmetrically and the other randomly. We characterize the nature of the thinking in these two strategies: the first we see as deterministic and the latter as stochastic, exploiting the random collisions of the ball. In this article we trace how the children’s thinking moved between these two perspectives.
Dave PrattEmail:
  相似文献   

5.
In this article we describe a mathematical microworld for investigating car motion on a racing course and its use with a group of grade 12 students. The microworld is concerned with the mathematical construction of courses and functions which describe car motion. It is implemented in the computer algebra system, Maple®, which provides the means to represent courses and functions symbolically and graphically. We describe the learning opportunities offered by the microworld in relation to the research literature on functions. Various facets and layers of the function concept are addressed in the microworld, and we suggest how work in the microworld might help in overcoming some well-known misconceptions.This revised version was published online in September 2005 with corrections to the Cover Date.  相似文献   

6.
Nathan and Arthur, 2 children in a 3-year teaching experiment on children's construction of the rational numbers of arithmetic (RNA), developed their operations for multiplying, dividing, and simplifying fractions over the last 2 years (Grades 4 and 5) of the experiment. The 2 children worked in the context of specially developed computer microworlds with a teacher/researcher for approximately 45 min a week for 50 weeks over the 2-year period. The children's construction of multiplicative fractional schemes was investigated in a retrospective analysis of each of the 50 videotaped teaching episodes. Four distinct modifications of the children's fractional schemes were discerned that contributed to their construction of the RNA. The investigation suggested that the operations and unit types associated with the children's whole-number sequences did not interfere with the reorganization of their fractional schemes but rather contributed to those schemes. The reorganization involved an integration of their whole-number knowledge with their fractional schemes whereby whole-number division was regarded as the same as multiplication by the reciprocal fraction.  相似文献   

7.
This article coordinates social constructivism and socioculturalism orientations to explain 2nd-grade children's reasoning with 2-digit quantities. From a social constructivist position, we illustrate how the classroom teacher and the students constituted what counted as an acceptable mathematical explanation. As children offered informal and conventional ways of interpreting problem situations, they were expected to reason with quantities in sensible ways. From a sociocultural position, we explain how the teacher's and students' contributions were situated within the mathematical ways of knowing constituted by the community at large. Particular children's contributions were clarified in terms of the ways in which they participated in socially organized activities. By coordinating these lenses, we argue the local classroom mathematical practices constrained and enabled the mathematical practices of the wider society.  相似文献   

8.
This case study discusses an activity that makes up one of five lessons in an ongoing classroom teaching experiment. The goal of the teaching experiment is (a) to gain insight into kindergartners’ spatial structuring abilities, and (b) to design an educational setting that can support kindergartners in becoming aware of spatial structures and in learning to apply spatial structuring as a means to abbreviate and ultimately elucidate numerical procedures. This paper documents children's spatial structuring of three-dimensional block constructions and the teacher's role in guiding the children's learning processes. The episodes have contributed to developing the activity into a lesson that could foster children's use of spatial structure for determining the number of blocks. The observations complement existing research that relates spatial structuring to mathematical performance, with additional insight into the development of number sense of particularly young children in a regular classroom setting.  相似文献   

9.
Drawing on a perspective of mathematics as situated social practice, we focus on 4 children in an urban preschool classroom and follow those children between home and school sites to shed light on urban children's persistent underachievement in mathematics. In this article, we describe the ways in which numeracy practices travel with children between home and school and, within those contexts, shape complex and sometimes limited social identities for children. We found that school imperatives, such as assessments and socialization curricula, often obscure teachers' views of children's mathematical practices. Deficit assumptions about family and community support for children, and limited interaction between caregivers and teachers, further contribute to the tendency of school personnel to overlook the mathematical practices that children bring with them to school. We further suggest that vignettes drawn from ethnographic-type research such as this have potential for professional development for classroom teachers.  相似文献   

10.
This study examines the mathematical processes used by students when solving physics tasks requiring proportional reasoning. The study investigates students' understanding and explanations of their mathematical processes. A qualitative and interpretive case study was conducted with 6 students from a coeducational urban high school for 5 months. Students were engaged with some high school physics tasks requiring proportional reasoning, during which a hermeneutic dialectic design was used to investigate their processes, understandings, and difficulties. Research techniques such as interviews, dialectical discourses, journal dialogue, and video and audio recordings were employed to generate, analyze, and interpret data. Results of the study indicate that the students employed mathematical proportional reasoning patterns and algorithms which they could not explain. Students also had difficulties translating physics tasks into mathematical statements, symbols, and relations. Students could not perform mathematical operations that were not directly obvious from the physics tasks, and some had difficulty with division. Students did not have adequate understanding of the mathematical processes involved in proportional reasoning.  相似文献   

11.
This study examined possible cultural and SES differences in the type, frequency, and complexity of 4- and 5-year-old American and Chinese children's everyday mathematical activities during free play. 60 American children, 30 each from lower- and middle-socioeconomic status (SES) families, and 24 Chinese children, 12 each from lower- and middle-SES families, participated in the study. Each participant was videotaped during free play for 15 min. The results showed that during free play American and Chinese children exhibit similar types of mathematical activity. Yet, Chinese children engage in a considerably greater amount of mathematical activity, particularly pattern and shape, than do American children. However, closer examination revealed that Chinese and American children do not differ in the complexity of mathematical activities. The results failed to reveal significant differences in the frequency and complexity of everyday mathematics between lower- and middle-SES groups in both cultures.  相似文献   

12.
Learning trajectories are presented of 2 fifth-grade children, Jason and Laura, who participated in the teaching experiment, Children's Construction of the Rational Numbers of Arithmetic. 5 teaching episodes were held with the 2 children, October 15 and November 1, 8, 15, and 22. During the fourth grade, the 2 children demonstrated distinctly different partitioning schemes-the equi-partitioning scheme (Jason) and the simultaneous partitioning scheme (Laura). At the outset of the children's fifth grade, it was hypothesized that the differences in the 2 schemes would be manifest in the children's production of fractions commensurate with a given fraction. During the October 15 teaching episode, Jason independently produced how much 3/4 of 1/4 of a stick was of the whole stick as a novelty, and it was inferred that he engaged in recursive partitioning operations. An analogous inference could not be made for Laura. The primary difference in the 2 children during the teaching episodes was Laura's dependency on Jason's independent explanations or actions to engage in the actions that were needed for her to be successful in explaining why a fraction such as 1/3 was commensurate to, say, 4/12.  相似文献   

13.
The human mind inevitably comprehends the world in mathematical terms (among others). Children's informal and invented mathematics contains on an implicit level many of the mathematical ideas that teachers want to promote on a formal and explicit level. These ideas may be innate, constructed for the purpose of adaptation, or picked up from an environment that is rich in mathematical structure, regardless of culture. Teachers should attempt to uncover the mathematical ideas contained in their students' thinking because much, but not all, of the mathematics curriculum is immanent in children's informal and invented knowledge. This mathematical perspective requires a focus not only on the child's constructive process but also on the mathematical content underlying the child's thinking. Teachers then can use these crude ideas as a foundation on which to construct a significant portion of classroom pedagogy. In doing this, teachers should recognize that children's invented strategies are not an end in themselves. Instead, the ultimate goal is to facilitate children's progressive mathematization of their immanent ideas. Children need to understand mathematics in deep, formal, and conventional ways.  相似文献   

14.
This paper presents a case study of Hugo’s construction of Euler diagrams to develop set-based meanings for the truth of mathematical conditionals. We use this case to set forth a framework of three stages of activity in students’ guided reinvention of mathematical logic: reading activity, connecting activity, and fluent activity. The framework also categorizes various forms of connecting activity by which students may reflect on their reading activity: connecting tasks, connecting representations, and connecting conditions for truth and falsehood (which we call meanings). We argue that coordinating such connections is necessary to justify logical equivalences, such as why contrapositive statements share truth-values. Through the case study, we document the representations and meanings that Hugo called upon to assign truth-values to conditionals. The framework should help clarify and advance future research on the teaching and learning of logic rooted in students’ mathematical activity.  相似文献   

15.
The concept of equivalence class plays a significant role in the structure of Rational Numbers. Piaget taught that in order to help elementary school children develop mathematical concepts, concrete objects and concrete reflection-enhancing-activities are needed. The “Shemesh” software was specially designed for learning equivalence-classes of fractions. The software offers concrete representations of such classes, as well as activities which cannot be constructed without a computer. In a discrete Cartesian system students construct points on the grid and learn to identify each such point as a fraction-numeral (a denominator-numerator pair). The children then learn to construct sets of such points, all of which are located on a line through the origin point. They learn to identify the line with the set of its constituent equivalent fractions. Subsequently, they investigate other phenomena and constructions in such systems, developing these constructions into additional fraction concepts. These concrete constructions can be used in solving traditional fraction problems as well as in broadening the scope of fraction meaning. Fifth-graders who used “Shemesh” in their learning activities were clinically interviewed several months after the learning sessions ended. These interviews revealed evidence indicating initial actual development of the desired mathematical concepts. This revised version was published online in July 2006 with corrections to the Cover Date.  相似文献   

16.
in this article, we discuss findings from a research study designed to characterize students' development of significant mathematical models by examining the shifts in their thinking that occur during problem investigations. These problem investigations were designed to elicit the development of mathematical models that can be used to describe and explain the relations, patterns, and structure found in data from experienced situations. We were particularly interested in a close examination of the student interactions that appear to foster the development of such mathematical models. This classroom-based qualitative case study was conducted with precalculus students enrolled in a moderate-sized private research university. We observed several groups of 3 students each as they worked together on 5 different modeling tasks. In each task, the students were asked to create a quantitative system that could describe and explain the patterns and structures in an experienced situation and that could be used to make predictions about the situation. Our analysis of the data revealed 4 sources of mismatches that were significant in bringing about the occurrence of shifts in student thinking: conjecturing, questioning, impasses to progress, and the use of technology-based representations. The shifts in thinking in turn led to the development of mathematical models. These results suggest that students would benefit from learning environments that provide them with ample opportunity to express their ideas, ask questions, make reasoned guesses, and work with technology while engaging in problem situations that elicit the development of significant mathematical models.  相似文献   

17.
in this article, we discuss findings from a research study designed to characterize students' development of significant mathematical models by examining the shifts in their thinking that occur during problem investigations. These problem investigations were designed to elicit the development of mathematical models that can be used to describe and explain the relations, patterns, and structure found in data from experienced situations. We were particularly interested in a close examination of the student interactions that appear to foster the development of such mathematical models. This classroom-based qualitative case study was conducted with precalculus students enrolled in a moderate-sized private research university. We observed several groups of 3 students each as they worked together on 5 different modeling tasks. In each task, the students were asked to create a quantitative system that could describe and explain the patterns and structures in an experienced situation and that could be used to make predictions about the situation. Our analysis of the data revealed 4 sources of mismatches that were significant in bringing about the occurrence of shifts in student thinking: conjecturing, questioning, impasses to progress, and the use of technology-based representations. The shifts in thinking in turn led to the development of mathematical models. These results suggest that students would benefit from learning environments that provide them with ample opportunity to express their ideas, ask questions, make reasoned guesses, and work with technology while engaging in problem situations that elicit the development of significant mathematical models.  相似文献   

18.
This paper introduces the language associated with a polygon microworld called PolygonR&D, which has the mathematical crispness of Logo and has the discreteness and simplicity of a Turing machine. In this microworld, polygons serve two purposes: as agents (similar to the turtles in Logo), and as data (landmarks in the plane). Programming the spatial behaviour of polygon agents is achieved by a simple variable-free language. Although limited in the number of instructions, the language allows for complex outcomes such as creation of sophisticated tilings, algorithm visualization, simulations, and even numerical computation. The ease of constructing the variety of examples shown in this paper indicates the microworld’s potential in both secondary and post-secondary education. An erratum to this article can be found at  相似文献   

19.
Children’s multiplicative thinking as the visualization of equal group structures and the enumeration the composite units was the subject of this study. The results were obtained from a small sample of Australian children (n = 18) in their first year of school (mean age 5 years 6 months) who participated in a lesson taught by their classroom teacher. The 12 Little Ducks problem stimulated children to visualize and to draw different ways of making equal groups. Fifteen children (83 %) could identify and create equal groups; eight of these children (44 %) could also quantify the number of groups they formed. These findings show that some young children understand early multiplicative ideas and can visualize equal group situations and communicate about these through their drawings and talk. The study emphasises the value of encouraging mathematical visualization from an early age; using open thought-provoking problems to reveal children’s thinking; and promoting drawing as a form of mathematical communication.  相似文献   

20.
This article describes the experiences gained from a seminar in the teaching of mathematical reasoning and problem solving designed to prepare in-service high school mathematics teachers to teach genuine mathematical activity in a computer-based environment. Presented with a set of unfamiliar tasks and activities, the participants were encouraged to investigate each of them, using the Geometer's Sketchpad software, and then to justify their assertions accordingly. In the exploratory process the student teachers make the major mathematical contributions while the teacher plays the role of facilitator. The mathematics teachers began to realize the power of technology in teaching mathematics and were pleased to return to their classrooms with a great number of experiences and ideas for immediate use.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号