首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Intensity discrimination thresholds for 500-ms pure-tone bursts were measured as a function of frequency in the goldfish (Carassius auratus) using classical respiratory conditioning. At 55-dB sensation level (SL), thresholds range from 1.44-2.2 dB between 100 and 1600 Hz. There is not important effect of frequency on intensity discrimination. Thresholds at 35-dB SL average 0.7 dB higher than at 55-dB SL. This is a small difference in the context of the threshold variability. In intensity discrimination acuity, the goldfish is quantitatively similar to other vertebrates, including birds and mammals.  相似文献   

2.
The acoustic frequency selectivity of over 500 saccular nerve fibers of the goldfish was studied using automated threshold tracking based on spike rate increments defined statistically. Saccular fibers of the goldfish show great variation in (1) best sensitivity (-26 to + 35 dB re: 1 dyn/cm2), (2) best frequency (below 100 to 1770 Hz), (3) spontaneous rate (0 to over 200 spikes/s), (4) spontaneous type (silent, regular, irregular, burst), and (5) degree of tuning (Q 10 dB from less than 0.1 to 2). Saccular fibers may be grouped into four nonoverlapping categories based on tuning and best frequency: (1) untuned (less than 10-dB variation in sensitivity between 100 and 1000 Hz), (2) low frequency (BF from below 120 to 290 Hz), (3) midfrequency (BF between 330 and 670 Hz), and (4) high frequency (BF between 790 and 1770 Hz). Within each category, all spontaneous rates and types, and all degrees of tuning can be observed. The least sensitive fibers within each group have zero spontaneous rates. The goldfish is like all other vertebrates studied in that the peripheral auditory system is adapted for frequency selectivity throughout the animal's entire frequency range of hearing. Peripheral tuning most likely accounts for behavioral determinations of the "auditory filter" and for the detectability of signals masked by noise. The signal-to-noise ratio enhancement provided by these peripheral filters is likely to be of primary biological significance. A "place principle" of sound quality analysis based on lines "labeled" according to best frequency in the brain cannot be ruled out on the basis of the peripheral physiology.  相似文献   

3.
The ability of goldfish to detect a change in the frequency of 400-Hz pure-tone bursts was studied using classical respiratory conditioning. The frequency discrimination threshold was measured at 15-, 35-, and 55-dB sensation level (SL), under conditions of (1) constant intensity, (2) roving intensity (plus and minus 6-dB burst-to-burst variation in intensity), (3) upward frequency change, and (4) downward frequency change. There was no overall effect of SL on frequency discrimination, but roving the intensity elevated thresholds by about 6 Hz (33%) and increased variability. Upward shifts in frequency elevated thresholds slightly (by 2 Hz or 10%) relative to downward shifts. These relatively small and statistically insignificant effects suggest that earlier measures of frequency discrimination in the goldfish are not due to the detection of simple changes in spike rate within individual peripheral channels.  相似文献   

4.
Classical conditioning of respiration was used to obtain psychometric functions for pulsed tone level discrimination in the goldfish (Carassius auratus). Conditioned respiratory suppression is a graded response that has some properties of a confidence rating measure. These properties were used to obtain receiver operating characteristics (ROC) and psychometric functions using a blocked method of constant stimuli. Empirical ROCs and neurometric functions were also obtained for single auditory-nerve fibers using spike count as the decision variable in order to evaluate a simple rate code for level discrimination. Psychometric and neurometric functions for level discrimination are similar in showing the same general form (summarized by Weibull functions) that is independent of signal duration. The lower slope of neurometric functions compared with behavioral functions for level discrimination is in accord with similar data on sound detection and vision in nonhuman mammals. Both neural and psychophysical level discrimination thresholds decline with increasing duration (20 to 320 ms), with similar slopes except at short signal durations (20 to 50 ms). At these durations, the animal's use of a channel-selection strategy and neural information following stimulus offset could reduce the difference between neural and psychophysical thresholds. The slopes of the neural and psychophysical duration functions are similar to those for human observers, but the majority of auditory-nerve fibers sampled have lower level discrimination thresholds than the behaving animal. Since human observers perform better than the majority of neurons in level discrimination, well-trained human listeners may be able to select channels with superior information, or to combine information across channels in ways that the goldfish and other animals do not. In general, one is encouraged to believe that neural mechanisms need not be more complex or sensitive than those considered here to account for pure-tone level discrimination in fishes, humans, and other vertebrates.  相似文献   

5.
The masking effects of white and amplitude comodulated noise were studied with respect to simple signal detection and sound source determination in goldfish. A stimulus generalization method was used to determine the signal-to-noise ratio required to completely determine the signal's characteristics. It was found that the S∕N required for this determination is about 4 dB greater than that required for signal detection, or was about 4 dB greater than the critical masking ratio. This means that the potential harm to fish of a given masking noise is at least 4 dB greater than previously thought, based on critical masking ratios. However, for amplitude comodulated noise between 10 and 50 Hz modulation rate, the potential harmful effects are up to 5.3 dB less than would be predicted from the critical masking ratio for unmodulated noise.  相似文献   

6.
A series of masking experiments was performed with the aim of comparing frequency selectivity for the monaural and binaural systems. The masking stimulus used in this study combined a sinusoid, which was gated simultaneously with the signal, with a continuous broadband noise. Signal frequency was fixed at 500 Hz. In one condition, the tonal masker and noise were interaurally in phase and the signal was phase reversed. In a second condition, noise, tonal masker, and signal were presented to one ear alone. Signal thresholds were obtained as a function of masker frequency for these two conditions. After making an appropriate selection of noise levels, masking functions for the monaural and binaural system conditions were found to agree closely except for a region about their tips where the binaural condition was more detectable. Two possible interpretations of these results are discussed. Either the monaural and binaural systems contain filters each which have similarly shaped skirts, or the frequency selectivity observed under both diotic and dichotic conditions (for large frequency separations of masker and signal) reflect the operation of a common peripheral filter.  相似文献   

7.
In a previous article [Lutfi, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 76, 1045-1050 (1984)], the following relation was used to predict measures of frequency selectivity obtained in forward masking from measures obtained in simultaneous masking: F(g) = G + H(g) - H(0), where, for a given masker level, F is the amount of forward masking (in dB) as a function of signal-masker frequency separation (g), H is the amount of simultaneous masking, and G is the amount of forward masking for g = 0. In the present study, the relation was tested for a wider range of signal and masker frequencies, masker levels, and signal delays. The relation described thresholds from all conditions well with the inclusion of one free parameter lambda corresponding to a constant frequency increment, F(g) = G + H(g + lambda) - H(lambda). The parameter lambda was required to account for observed shifts in the frequency of maximum forward masking. It is argued that a single tuning mechanism can account for commonly observed differences between simultaneous- and forward-masked measures of frequency selectivity.  相似文献   

8.
The purpose of this study was to compare the role of frequency selectivity in measures of auditory and vibrotactile temporal resolution. In the first experiment, temporal modulation transfer functions for a sinusoidally amplitude modulated (SAM) 250-Hz carrier revealed auditory modulation thresholds significantly lower than corresponding vibrotactile modulation thresholds at SAM frequencies greater than or equal to 100 Hz. In the second experiment, auditory and vibrotactile gap detection thresholds were measured by presenting silent gaps bounded by markers of the same or different frequency. The marker frequency F1 = 250 Hz preceded the silent gap and marker frequencies after the silent gap included F2 = 250, 255, 263, 310, and 325 Hz. Auditory gap detection thresholds were lower than corresponding vibrotactile thresholds for F2 markers less than or equal to 263 Hz, but were greater than the corresponding vibrotactile gap detection thresholds for F2 markers greater than or equal to 310 Hz. When the auditory gap detection thresholds were transformed into filter attenuation values, the results were modeled well by a constant-percentage (10%) bandwidth filter centered on F1. The vibrotactile gap detection thresholds, however, were independent of marker frequency separation. In a third experiment, auditory and vibrotactile rate difference limens (RDLs) were measured for a 250-Hz carrier at SAM rates less than or equal to 100 Hz. Auditory RDLs were lower than corresponding vibrotactile RDLs for standard rates greater than 10 Hz. Combination tones may have confounded auditory performance for standard rates of 80 and 100 Hz. The results from these experiments revealed that frequency selectivity influences auditory measures of temporal resolution, but there was no evidence of frequency selectivity affecting vibrotactile temporal resolution.  相似文献   

9.
Two experiments are described in which frequency selectivity was estimated, in simultaneous and forward masking, for each ear of subjects with moderate (25-60 dB HL) unilateral cochlear hearing losses. In both experiments, the signal level was fixed for a given ear and type of masking (simultaneous or forward), and the masker level was varied to determine threshold, using an adaptive, two-alternative forced-choice procedure. In experiment I, the masker was a noise with a spectral notch centered at the signal frequency (either 1.0 or 1.5 kHz); threshold was determined as a function of notch width. Signal levels were chosen so that the noise level required at threshold for a notch width of zero was similar for the normal and impaired ear of each subject in both simultaneous and forward masking. The function relating threshold to notch width had a steeper slope for the normal ear than for the impaired ear of each subject. For the normal ears, these functions were steeper in forward masking than in simultaneous masking. This difference was interpreted as resulting from suppression. For the impaired ears, significant differences in the same direction were observed for three of the five subjects, but the differences were smaller. In experiment II, psychophysical tuning curves (PTCs) were determined in the presence of a fixed notched noise centered at the signal frequency (1.0 kHz). For the normal ears, the PTCs were sharper in forward masking than in simultaneous masking. For the impaired ears, the PTCs were similar in simultaneous and forward masking, but those in forward masking tended to be sharper at masker frequencies far removed from the signal frequency. Overall, the results suggest that suppression is reduced, but not completely absent in cases of moderate cochlear hearing loss.  相似文献   

10.
Previous studies have shown that threshold for a signal in tone-on-tone simultaneous masking is sometimes lower when the masker is continuous than when it is gated. Threshold may also decline as signal onset is delayed relative to the onset of a longer duration masker, though it may increase again near masker offset. In the present study, the level of a 1250-Hz sinusoidal masker was found which would just mask a 20-ms, 1000-Hz sinusoid presented at 10-dB sensation level (SL). Masker duration was 20 or 400 ms; in the latter case, the signal was presented in one of three temporal positions within the masker. The level of the 1250-Hz masker necessary to mask the signal was reduced, sometimes by as much as 20-25 dB, by a 20-ms, 500-Hz sinusoid (transient masker) presented at the times when the signal might occur, but at a level 30 dB below that at which it would mask the 10-dB SL signal. This suggests that, in the earlier studies, at least some of the elevation in threshold in the presence of a short-duration masker or at the beginning (or end) of a longer duration masker may have been due to the transient responses to the masker affecting detection of the signal, but not necessarily masking the signal in terms of excitation in the signal "channel."  相似文献   

11.
12.
The temporal course of simultaneous tone-on-tone masking   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Threshold for a 20-ms, 1-kHz signal was measured as a function of its temporal position within a longer duration gated masker; masker frequencies were below, at, and above 1 kHz. For a masker frequency above the signal frequency, there is a sizable temporal effect: As the onset of the signal is delayed, threshold decreases rapidly but then increases again as the signal approaches masker offset. Similar results can be observed for a masker frequency below the signal frequency, but that temporal effect is due to the detection of the cubic difference tone. The implication of this frequency-dependent temporal effect for measuring psychophysical tuning curves is discussed.  相似文献   

13.
The temporal course of masking and the auditory filter shape   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Recent experiments have shown that frequency selectivity measured in tone-on-tone simultaneous masking improves with increasing delay of a brief signal relative to the onset of a longer duration gated masker. To determine whether a similar improvement occurs for a notched-noise masker, threshold was measured for a 20-ms signal presented at the beginning, the temporal center, or the end of the 400-ms masker (simultaneous masking), or immediately following the masker (forward masking). The notch width was varied systematically and the notch was placed both symmetrically and asymmetrically about the 1-kHz signal frequency. Growth-of-masking functions were determined for each temporal condition, for a noise masker without a spectral notch. These functions were used to express the thresholds from the notched-noise experiment in terms of the level of a flat-spectrum noise which would produce the same threshold. In simultaneous masking the auditory filter shapes derived from the transformed data did not change significantly with signal delay, suggesting that the selectivity of the auditory filter does not develop over time. In forward masking the auditory filter shapes were sharper than those for simultaneous masking, particularly on the high-frequency side, which was attributed to suppression.  相似文献   

14.
The relation between the monaural critical band and binaural analysis was examined using an NoSm MLD paradigm, in order to resolve ambiguities about the width of the masking spectrum important for binaural detection. A 500-Hz pure-tone signal was presented with a 600-Hz-wide band of masking noise to the signal ear. Bands of noise ranging in width from 25 to 600 Hz, or noise notches (imposed on a 600-Hz-wide band centered on the signal frequency) ranging in width from 0 to 600 Hz were presented to the nonsignal ear. All noise bands and notches were centered on 500 Hz, the frequency of the signal. The effects of varying bandwidth were radically different from those of varying notchwidth: the MLD changed from zero to approximately 8 dB over a bandwidth range of 400 Hz; for notchwidths, however, the MLD changed 8 dB over a range of only 50 Hz. The results support an interpretation that the fine frequency selectivity of monaural analysis is preserved in peripheral binaural interaction, but that a relatively wide frequency range of critical bands is scanned at a later stage of binaural processing. It was suggested that the wide spectral range of binaural analysis may provide a background against which binaural differences due to the signal are detected.  相似文献   

15.
Temporal fine structure (TFS) sensitivity, frequency selectivity, and speech reception in noise were measured for young normal-hearing (NHY), old normal-hearing (NHO), and hearing-impaired (HI) subjects. Two measures of TFS sensitivity were used: the "TFS-LF test" (interaural phase difference discrimination) and the "TFS2 test" (discrimination of harmonic and frequency-shifted tones). These measures were not significantly correlated with frequency selectivity (after partialing out the effect of audiometric threshold), suggesting that insensitivity to TFS cannot be wholly explained by a broadening of auditory filters. The results of the two tests of TFS sensitivity were significantly but modestly correlated, suggesting that performance of the tests may be partly influenced by different factors. The NHO group performed significantly more poorly than the NHY group for both measures of TFS sensitivity, but not frequency selectivity, suggesting that TFS sensitivity declines with age in the absence of elevated audiometric thresholds or broadened auditory filters. When the effect of mean audiometric threshold was partialed out, speech reception thresholds in modulated noise were correlated with TFS2 scores, but not measures of frequency selectivity or TFS-LF test scores, suggesting that a reduction in sensitivity to TFS can partly account for the speech perception difficulties experienced by hearing-impaired subjects.  相似文献   

16.
The detection of 500- or 2000-Hz pure-tone signals in unmodulated and modulated noise was investigated in normal-hearing and sensorineural hearing-impaired listeners, as a function of noise bandwidth. Square-wave modulation rates of 15 and 40 Hz were used in the modulated noise conditions. A notched noise measure of frequency selectivity and a gap detection measure of temporal resolution were also obtained on each subject. The modulated noise results indicated a masking release that increased as a function of increasing noise bandwidth, and as a function of decreasing modulation rate for both groups of listeners. However, the improvement of threshold with increasing modulated noise bandwidth was often greatly reduced among the sensorineural hearing-impaired listeners. It was hypothesized that the masking release in modulated noise may be due to several types of processes including across-critical band analysis (CMR), within-critical band analysis, and suppression. Within-band effects appeared to be especially large at the higher frequency region and lower modulation rate. In agreement with previous research, there was a significant correlation between frequency selectivity and masking release in modulated noise. At the 500-Hz region, masking release was correlated more highly with the filter skirt and tail measures than with the filter passband measure. At the 2000-Hz region, masking release was correlated more with the filter passband and skirt measures than with the filter tail measure. The correlation between gap detection and masking release was significant at the 40-Hz modulation rate, but not at the 15-Hz modulation rate. The results of this study suggest that masking release in modulated noise is limited by frequency selectivity at low modulation rates, and by both frequency selectivity and temporal resolution at high modulation rates. However, even when the present measures of frequency selectivity and temporal resolution are both taken into account, significant variance in masking release still remains unaccounted for.  相似文献   

17.
We present an application of a growth model for a system of cities. This computer model simulates the evolution of systems with measurable entities (e.g. city size), and takes into account the growth of the entities in terms of size and number. It includes a random multiplicative process for the growth of individual entities and for the creation of new ones. We use a new mathematical expression with a positive exponent α (which we call the ‘shape exponent’) and additional three parameters, to describe the dynamics of the systems’ size distributions through time. We compare the changes of a real system of cities and the model’s results quantitatively and qualitatively. Our findings suggest that there is a good agreement at the macro level between the model and the real data.  相似文献   

18.
An experiment tested the hypothesis that the masking effects of two nonoverlapping forward maskers are summed linearly over time. First, the levels of individual noise maskers required to mask a brief 4-kHz signal presented at 10-, 20-, 30-, or 40-dB sensation level (SL) were found. The hypothesis predicts that a combination of the first masker presented at the level required to mask the 10-dB SL signal and the second masker presented at the level required to mask the 20-dB SL signal, should produce the same amount of masking as the converse situation (i.e., the first masker presented at the level required to mask the 20-dB SL signal and the second masker presented at the level required to mask the 10-dB SL signal), and similarly for the 30- and 40-dB SL signals. The results were consistent with the predictions.  相似文献   

19.
A simultaneous masking procedure was used to derive four measures of frequency selectivity in the chinchilla. The first experiment measured critical masking ratios (CRs) at various signal frequencies. Estimates of the chinchillas' critical bandwidths derived from the CRs were much broader than comparable human estimates, indicating that the chinchilla may have inferior frequency selectivity. The second experiment measured critical bandwidths at 1, 2, and 4 kHz in a band-narrowing experiment. This technique yielded narrower estimates of critical bandwidth; however, chinchillas continued to exhibit poor frequency selectivity compared to man. The third experiment measured auditory-filter shape at 0.5, 1, and 2 kHz via rippled noise masking. Results of the rippled noise masking experiment indicate that auditory filters of humans and chinchillas are similar in terms of shape and bandwidth with chinchillas showing only slightly poorer frequency selectivity. The final experiment measured auditory filter shape at 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz using notched noise masking. This experiment yielded auditory filter shapes and bandwidths similar to those derived from man. The discrepancy between the indirect estimates of frequency selectivity derived from CR and band-narrowing techniques and the direct estimates derived from rippled noise and notched noise masking are explained by taking into account the processing efficiency of the subjects.  相似文献   

20.
It is widely accepted from noise models that the extracted performance parameters such as spatial and temporal NEDT do not depend on the number of samples, which are used to estimate the noise values. Experimental studies have determined that noise values depend on the number of sampled data because of other noise contribution or its frequency dependence. This, however, creates ambiguities since unique values NEDT cannot be established without fixing the number of frames to be utilized. However in the frequency domain, values of parameters can be easily established at certain frequency. In the frequency domain it is also easier to study the noise contribution originating from various noise sources such as from the ROIC. This presentation will present preliminary analysis of spatial and temporal noise in the frequency domain by utilizing the power of FFT analysis.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号