首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Because they consist, in large part, of random turbulent noise, fricatives present a challenge to attempts to specify the phonetic correlates of phonological features. Previous research has focused on temporal properties, acoustic power, and a variety of spectral properties of fricatives in a number of contexts [Jongman et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108, 1252-1263 (2000); Jesus and Shadle, J. Phonet. 30, 437-467 (2002); Crystal and House, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 1553-1573 (1988a)]. However, no systematic investigation of the effects of focus and prosodic context on fricative production has been carried out. Manipulation of explicit focus can serve to selectively exaggerate linguistically relevant properties of speech in much the same manner as stress [de Jong, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 97, 491-504 (1995); de Jong, J. Phonet. 32, 493-516 (2004); de Jong and Zawaydeh, J. Phonet. 30, 53-75 (2002)]. This experimental technique was exploited to investigate acoustic power along with temporal and spectral characteristics of American English fricatives in two prosodic contexts, to probe whether native speakers selectively attend to subsegmental features, and to consider variability in fricative production across speakers. While focus in general increased noise power and duration, speakers did not selectively enhance spectral features of the target fricatives.  相似文献   

2.
Models of the additivity of masking   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   

3.
Kewley-Port, Watson, and Foyle [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 1133-1145 (1988)] describe a study that uses several different procedures to measure thresholds for stimuli whose components differ in temporal onset. Unfortunately, misunderstandings and misconceptions (shared with other recent publications) resulted in conclusions that are both unnecessary and unwarranted. The Kewley-Port et al. article is discussed in terms of often replicated published findings on temporal order thresholds, and current misconceptions of perceptual concepts and models.  相似文献   

4.
Cochlear model calculations are shown to be in reasonable agreement with recent low-frequency measurements of intracochlear pressures and the cochlear input impedance of the cat [V. Nedzelnitsky, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 68, 1676-1689 (1980); T. J. Lynch, III, V. Nedzelnitsky, and W. T. Peake, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 72, 108-130 (1982)]. Included in the cochlear model are perilymph viscosity, the measured variation of the area of the scala vestibuli with distance from the stapes [P. Dallos, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 48, 489-499 (1970)], and finite impedance of the round window membrane. The WKB approximation and its extension to the low-frequency region is used in order to exhibit explicitly the dependence of the model results on the cochlear parameters.  相似文献   

5.
On the interpretability of speech/nonspeech comparisons: a reply to Fowler   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Fowler [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 88, 1236-1249 (1990)] makes a set of claims on the basis of which she denies the general interpretability of experiments that compare the perception of speech sounds to the perception of acoustically analogous nonspeech sound. She also challenges a specific auditory hypothesis offered by Diehl and Walsh [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 2154-2164 (1989)] to explain the stimulus-length effect in the perception of stops and glides. It will be argued that her conclusions are unwarranted.  相似文献   

6.
Schroeder [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 79, 186-189 (1986)] describes the paradox of acoustic waveforms that sound lower when reproduced at higher speeds. The author has also demonstrated this paradox [J.C. Risset, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 46, 88 (A) (1969); see also Seventh ICA, Budapest, S10, 613-616 (1971)]; in addition he has recently synthesized a rhythmic analog of the paradox, namely rhythmical sequences that can sound slower when reproduced at higher speeds.  相似文献   

7.
Listeners' ability to understand speech in adverse listening conditions is partially due to the redundant nature of speech. Natural redundancies are often lost or altered when speech is filtered, such as done in AI/SII experiments. It is important to study how listeners recognize speech when the speech signal is unfiltered and the entire broadband spectrum is present. A correlational method [R. A. Lutfi, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 97, 1333-1334 (1995); V. M. Richards and S. Zhu, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 95, 423-424 (1994)] has been used to determine how listeners use spectral cues to perceive nonsense syllables when the full speech spectrum is present [K. A. Doherty and C. W. Turner, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 3769-3773 (1996); C. W. Turner et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 104, 1580-1585 (1998)]. The experiments in this study measured spectral-weighting strategies for more naturally occurring speech stimuli, specifically sentences, using a correlational method for normal-hearing listeners. Results indicate that listeners placed the greatest weight on spectral information within bands 2 and 5 (562-1113 and 2807-11,000 Hz), respectively. Spectral-weighting strategies for sentences were also compared to weighting strategies for nonsense syllables measured in a previous study (C. W. Turner et al., 1998). Spectral-weighting strategies for sentences were different from those reported for nonsense syllables.  相似文献   

8.
Comodulation masking release and the masking-level difference   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
An experiment was performed to determine if the mechanism that mediates comodulation masking release (CMR) is associated with that used to improve detection by the masking-level difference (MLD). The experiment consisted of first improving detectability of a masked diotic tone burst by adding a synchronous noise band at another frequency region (CMR), and then measuring an MLD in the usual manner, by inverting the tone-burst signal to one ear. Results indicate that a substantial MLD can be measured for a signal whose detectability has already been improved by CMR. However, that MLD (9 dB) is smaller than that measured in random noise (14 dB). Put another way, a small CMR (4 dB) can be produced even when the detectability of a stimulus has already been improved due to the MLD. These data are in general agreement with those of Hall et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 1839-1845 (1988)] and Schooneveldt and Moore [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 262-272 (1989)].  相似文献   

9.
The study by Brenkman et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 82, 1646-1654 (1987)] of malleus umbo and anterior crus of stapes displacement in 14 human temporal bones shows a mean -7.3-dB/oct slope above 1.0 kHz for stapes displacement in response to a 80-dB SPL input at the eardrum. The slope they obtained for midfrequency (1.0-4.0 kHz) stapes displacement is significantly flatter than what was found previously [Gyo et al., Acta Otolaryngol. 103, 87-95 (1987); Gundersen, Prostheses in the Ossicular Chain (University Park, Baltimore, MD, 1971); Kringlebotn and Gundersen, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 77, 159-164 (1985); Vlaming and Feenstra, Clin. Otolaryngol. 11, 353-363 (1986a)]; in these studies, stapes displacement rolled off at -12.0 to -14.9 dB/oct above 1.0 kHz. It appears that their mean midfrequency stapes displacement slope has been flattened by some unusual results in a small number of ears. Possible reasons for these results are discussed.  相似文献   

10.
Recent improvements in the parabolic equation method are combined to extend this approach to a larger class of seismo-acoustics problems. The variable rotated parabolic equation [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 120, 3534-3538 (2006)] handles a sloping fluid-solid interface at the ocean bottom. The single-scattering solution [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 121, 808-813 (2007)] handles range dependence within elastic sediment layers. When these methods are implemented together, the parabolic equation method can be applied to problems involving variations in bathymetry and the thickness of sediment layers. The accuracy of the approach is demonstrated by comparing with finite-element solutions. The approach is applied to a complex scenario in a realistic environment.  相似文献   

11.
Time domain cochlear models have primarily followed a method introduced by Allen and Sondhi [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 66, 123-132 (1979)]. Recently the "state space formalism" proposed by Elliott et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 122, 2759-2771 (2007)] has been used to simulate a wide range of nonlinear cochlear models. It used a one-dimensional approach that is extended to two dimensions in this paper, using the finite element method. The recently developed "state space formalism" in fact shares a close relationship to the earlier approach. Working from Diependaal et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 82, 1655-1666 (1987)] the two approaches are compared and the relationship formalized. Understanding this relationship allows models to be converted from one to the other in order to utilize each of their strengths. A second method to derive the state space matrices required for the "state space formalism" is also presented. This method offers improved numerical properties because it uses the information available about the model more effectively. Numerical results support the claims regarding fluid dimension and the underlying similarity of the two approaches. Finally, the recent advances in the state space formalism [Bertaccini and Sisto, J. Comp. Phys. 230, 2575-2587 (2011)] are discussed in terms of this relationship.  相似文献   

12.
Simultaneous multiple acoustical sources measurement (SMASM) has been proposed for more effective and reliable identification of acoustical systems under critical conditions [N. Xiang and M. R. Schroeder, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113, 2754-2761 (2003); N. Xiang, J. N. Daigle, and M. Kleiner, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 117, 1889-1894 (2005)]. This paper presents a pseudo-inverse algorithm for the SMASM correlation technique as an alternative way of extracting impulse responses of acoustical channels. Simulations and room acoustics experiments are carried out and the results prove the feasibility of the proposed algorithm.  相似文献   

13.
The phenomenological framework outlined in the companion paper [C. A. Shera and G. Zweig, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 92, 1356-1370 (1992)] characterizes both forward and reverse transmission through the middle ear. This paper illustrates its use in the analysis of noninvasive measurements of middle-ear and cochlear mechanics. A cochlear scattering framework is developed for the analysis of combination-tone and other experiments in which acoustic distortion products are used to drive the middle ear "in reverse." The framework is illustrated with a simple psychophysical Gedankenexperiment analogous to the neurophysiological experiments of P. F. Fahey and J. B. Allen [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 77, 599-612 (1985)].  相似文献   

14.
The acoustic behavior in thermo-viscous gas mixtures, both in proximity of walls and far from them (outside the boundary layers), involves deviations from the adiabatic and laminar movements in pure gases, which result from the influence of several diffusive fields, namely, shear, entropic, and concentration variation fields (their energy being provided by the acoustic field itself). Owing to the boundary conditions, that are slip condition, isothermal condition and concentration flux vanishing on the walls, a strong coupling between these fields occurs inside the boundary layers while their effects appear to be simple additive processes in the bulk of the medium. Although recent literature on this subject leads to interesting results, opening the way to several new issues [R. Raspet et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105, 65-73 (1999); R. Raspet et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 112, 1414-1422 (2002); G. W. Swift and P. S. Spoor, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 106, 1794-1800 (1999); D. A. Geller and G. W. Swift, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 111, 1675-1684 (2002)], the results available still have limitations because they do not provide complete solutions for the propagative and diffusive fields throughout and beyond the boundary layers. The present work aims at providing these solutions in the whole domains considered. The results allow interpreting analytically the behavior of the fields above mentioned in closed cavities and ducts, and particularly in spherical cavities which are best suited to develop metrological applications.  相似文献   

15.
Chotiros and Isakson [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 116(4), 2011-2022 (2004)] recently proposed an extension of the Biot-Stoll model for poroelastic sediments that makes predictions for compressional wave speed and attenuation, which are in much better accord with the experimental measurements of these quantities extant in the literature than either those of the conventional Biot-Stoll model or the rival model of Buckingham [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108(6), 2796-2815 (2000)]. Using a local minimizer, the Nelder-Mead simplex method, it is shown that there are generally at least two choices of the Chotiros-Isakson parameters which produce good agreement with experimental measurements. Since one postulate of the Chotiros-Isakson model is that, due to the presence of air bubbles in the pore space, the pore fluid compressibility is greater than that of water, an alternative model based on a conjecture by Biot [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 34(5), 1254-1264 (1962)], air bubble resonance, is considered. While this model does as well or better than the Chotiros-Isakson model in predicting measured values of wave speed and attenuation, the Rayleigh-Plesset theory of bubble oscillation casts doubt on its plausibility as a general explanation of large dispersion of velocity with respect to frequency.  相似文献   

16.
Binaural disparities are the primary acoustic cues employed in sound localization tasks. However, the degree of binaural correlation in a sound serves as a complementary cue for detecting competing sound sources [J. F. Culling, H. S. Colburn, and M. Spurchise, "Interaural correlation sensitivity," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110(2), 1020-1029 (2001) and L. R. Bernstein and C. Trahiotis, "On the use of the normalized correlation as an index of interaural envelope correlation," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 1754-1763 (1996)]. Here a random chord stereogram (RCS) sound is developed that produces a salient pop-out illusion of a slowly varying ripple sound [T. Chi et al., "Spectro-temporal modulation transfer functions and speech intelligibility," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 106(5), 2719-2732 (1999)], even though the left and right ear sounds alone consist of noise-like random modulations. The quality and resolution of this percept is systematically controlled by adjusting the spectrotemporal correlation pattern between the left and right sounds. The prominence and limited time-frequency resolution for resolving the RCS suggests that envelope correlations are a dominant binaural cue for grouping acoustic objects.  相似文献   

17.
The effects of coupled vibrations on the piezoelectric properties and electroacoustic transducer performance of radially polarized hollow cylindrical elements as a function of the choice of height-to-diameter ratio have been well documented [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 120(3), 1374-1380 (2006); J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 122(6), 3419-3427 (2007)]. This letter presents experimental work on the effects of coupled (circumferential, axial, and flexural) vibrations on the resonance frequencies, effective electromechanical coupling coefficient, and acoustical performance of tangentially polarized piezoceramic cylindrical elements. Comparisons are made with analytical predictions of the properties of uniformly circumferentially polarized cylinders.  相似文献   

18.
Models of auditory masking: a molecular psychophysical approach   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Gilkey et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 78, 1207-1219 (1985)] measured hit proportions and false alarm proportions for detecting a 500-Hz tone at each of four starting phase angles in each of 25 reproducible noise samples. In the present paper, their results are modeled by fitting the general form of the electrical analog model of Jeffress [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 48, 480-488 (1967)] to the diotic data. The best-fitting configurations of this model do not correspond to energy detectors or to envelope detectors. A detector composed of a 50-Hz-wide single-tuned filter, followed by a half-wave rectifier and an integrator with an integration time of 100 to 200 ms fits the data of all four subjects relatively well. Linear combinations of the outputs of several detectors that differ in center frequency or integration window provide even better fits to the data. These linear combinations assign negative weights to some frequencies or to some time intervals, suggesting that a subject's decision is based on a comparison of information in different spectral or temporal portions of the stimulus.  相似文献   

19.
In our preliminary theory of intensity resolution [e.g., see N. I. Durlach and L. D. Braida, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 46, 372-383 (1969)], two modes of memory operation are postulated: the trace mode and the context-coding mode. In this paper, we present a revised model of the context-coding mode which describes explicitly a process by which sensations are coded relative to the context and which predicts a resolution edge effect [L. D. Braida and N. I. Durlach, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 51, 483-502 (1972); J. E. Berliner, L. D. Braida, and N. I. Durlach, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 61, 1256-1267 (1977)]. The sensation arising from a given stimulus presentation is coded by determining its distance from internal references or perceptual anchors. The noise in this process, combined with the sensation noise, constitutes the limitation on resolution in the model. In the revised model the probability density functions of the decision variable are not precisely Gaussian (and cannot be expressed analytically in closed form). This paper outlines the predictions of the model for one-interval paradigms and for fixed-level two-interval paradigms and derives estimates of the values of model parameters.  相似文献   

20.
A model of peripheral auditory processing that incorporates processing steps describing the conversion from the acoustic pressure-wave signal at the eardrum to the time course activity in auditory neurons has been developed. It can process arbitrary time domain waveforms and yield the probability of neural firing. The model consists of a concatenation of modules, one for each anatomical section of the periphery. All modules are based on published algorithms and current experimental data, except that the basilar membrane is assumed to be linear. The responses of this model to vowels alone and vowels in noise are compared to neural population responses, as determined by the temporal and average rate response measures of Sachs and Young [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 66, 470-479, (1979)] and Young and Sachs [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 66, 1381-1403, (1979)]. Despite the exclusion of nonlinear membrane mechanics, the model accurately predicts the vowel formant representations in the average localized synchronized rate (ALSR) responses and the saturating characteristics of the normalized average rate responses in quiet. When vowels are presented in background noise, the modeled ALSR responses are less robust than the neural data.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号