首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Two experiments investigated the role of the regularity of the frequency spacing of harmonics, as a separate factor from harmonicity, on the perception of the virtual pitch of a harmonic series. The first experiment compared the shifts produced by mistuning the 3rd, 4th, and 5th harmonics in the pitch of two harmonic series: the odd-H and the all-H tones. The odd-H tone contained odd harmonics 1 to 11, plus the 4th harmonic; the all-H tone contained harmonics 1 to 12. Both tones had a fundamental frequency of 155 Hz. Pitch shifts produced by mistuning the 3rd harmonic, but not the 4th and 5th harmonics, were found to be significantly larger for the odd-H tone than for the all-H tone. This finding was consistent with the idea that grouping by spectral regularity affects pitch perception since an odd harmonic made a larger contribution than an adjacent even harmonic to the pitch of the odd-H tone. However, an alternative explanation was that the 3rd mistuned harmonic produced larger pitch shifts within the odd-H tone than the 4th mistuned harmonic because of differences in the partial masking of these harmonics by adjacent harmonics. The second experiment tested these explanations by measuring pitch shifts for a modified all-H tone in which each mistuned odd harmonic was tested in the presence of the 4th harmonic, but in the absence of its other even-numbered neighbor. The results showed that, for all mistuned harmonics, pitch shifts for the modified all-H tone were not significantly different from those for the odd-H tone. These findings suggest that the harmonic relations among frequency components, rather than the regularity of their frequency spacing, is the primary factor for the perception of the virtual pitch of complex sounds.  相似文献   

2.
The integration of nonsimultaneous frequency components into a single virtual pitch was investigated by using a pitch matching task in which a mistuned 4th harmonic (mistuned component) produced pitch shifts in a harmonic series (12 equal-amplitude harmonics of a 155-Hz F0). In experiment 1, the mistuned component could either be simultaneous, stop as the target started (pre-target component), or start as the target stopped (post-target component). Pitch shifts produced by the pre-target components were significantly smaller than those obtained with simultaneous components; in the post-target condition, the size of pitch shifts did not decrease relative to the simultaneous condition. In experiment 2, a silent gap of 20, 40, 80, or 160 ms was introduced between the nonsimultaneous components and the target sound. In the pre-target condition, pitch shifts were reduced to zero for silent gaps of 80 ms or longer; by contrast, a gap of 160 ms was required to eliminate pitch shifts in the post-target condition. The third experiment tested the hypothesis that, when post-target components were presented, the processing of the pitch of the target tone started at the onset of the target, and ended at the gap duration at which pitch shifts decreased to zero. This hypothesis was confirmed by the finding that pitch shifts could not be observed when the target tone had a duration of 410 ms. Taken together, the results of these experiments show that nonsimultaneous components that occur after the onset of the target sound make a larger contribution to the virtual pitch of the target, and over a longer period, than components that precede the onset of the target sound.  相似文献   

3.
Ciocca and Darwin [V. Ciocca and C. J. Darwin, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105, 2421-2430 (1999)] reported that the shift in residue pitch caused by mistuning a single harmonic (the fourth out of the first 12) was the same when the mistuned harmonic was presented after the remainder of the complex as when it was simultaneous, even though subjects were asked to ignore the pure-tone percept. The present study tried to replicate this result, and investigated the role of the presence of the nominally mistuned harmonic in the matching sound. Subjects adjusted a "matching" sound so that its pitch equaled that of a subsequent 90-ms complex tone (12 harmonics of a 155-Hz F0), whose mistuned (+/-3%) third harmonic was presented either simultaneously with or after the remaining harmonics. In experiment 1, the matching sound was a harmonic complex whose third harmonic was either present or absent. In experiments 2A and 2B, the target and matching sound had nonoverlapping spectra. Pitch shifts were reduced both when the mistuned component was nonsimultaneous, and when the third harmonic was absent in the matching sound. The results indicate a shorter than originally estimated time window for obligatory integration of nonsimultaneous components into a virtual pitch.  相似文献   

4.
When a low harmonic in a harmonic complex tone is mistuned from its harmonic value by a sufficient amount it is heard as a separate tone, standing out from the complex as a whole. This experiment estimated the degree of mistuning required for this phenomenon to occur, for complex tones with 10 or 12 equal-amplitude components (60 dB SPL per component). On each trial the subject was presented with a complex tone which either had all its partials at harmonic frequencies or had one partial mistuned from its harmonic frequency. The subject had to indicate whether he heard a single complex tone with one pitch or a complex tone plus a pure tone which did not "belong" to the complex. An adaptive procedure was used to track the degree of mistuning required to achieve a d' value of 1. Threshold was determined for each ot the first six harmonics of each complex tone. In one set of conditions stimulus duration was held constant at 410 ms, and the fundamental frequency was either 100, 200, or 400 Hz. For most conditions the thresholds fell between 1% and 3% of the harmonic frequency, depending on the subject. However, thresholds tended to be greater for the first two harmonics of the 100-Hz fundamental and, for some subjects, thresholds increased for the fifth and sixth harmonics. In a second set of conditions fundamental frequency was held constant at 200 Hz, and the duration was either 50, 110, 410, or 1610 ms. Thresholds increased by a factor of 3-5 as duration was decreased from 1610 ms to 50 ms. The results are discussed in terms of a hypothetical harmonic sieve and mechanisms for the formation of perceptual streams.  相似文献   

5.
Thresholds were measured for the detection of inharmonicity in complex tones. Subjects were required to distinguish a complex tone whose partials were all at exact harmonic frequencies from a similar complex tone with one of the partials slightly mistuned. The mistuning which allowed 71% correct identification in a two-alternative forced-choice task was estimated for each partial in turn. In experiment I the fundamental frequency was either 100, 200, or 400 Hz, and the complex tones contained the first 12 harmonics at equal levels of 60 dB SPL per component. The stimulus duration was 410 ms. For each fundamental the thresholds were roughly constant when expressed in Hz, having a mean value of about 4 Hz (range 2.4-7.3 Hz). In experiment II the fundamental frequency was fixed at 200 Hz, and thresholds for inharmonicity were measured for stimulus durations of 50, 110, 410, and 1610 ms. For harmonics above the fifth the thresholds increased from less than 1 Hz to about 40 Hz as duration was decreased from 1610-50 ms. For the lower harmonics (up to the fourth) threshold changed much less with duration, and for the three shorter durations thresholds for each duration were roughly a constant proportion of the harmonic frequency. The results suggest that inharmonicity is detected in different ways for high and low harmonics. For low harmonics the inharmonic partial appears to "stand out" from the complex tone as a whole. For high harmonics the mistuning is detected as a kind of "beat" or "roughness," presumably reflecting a sensitivity to the changing relative phase of the mistuned harmonic relative to the other harmonics.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

6.
Envelope-induced pitch shifts were measured for exponentially decaying complex tones consisting of two sinusoidal components with frequencies f1 = nf0 + 50 Hz and f2 = (n + 1) f0 + 50 Hz, where n equals 3, 4, or 5 and exponential decay rates were 0, 0.5, 1, and 2 dB/ms. Four subjects adjusted a sinusoidal comparison tone to match the virtual pitch of the (missing) fundamental and the pitches of the lower and upper partials f1 and f2. Pitch shifts for f1 are generally less, and pitch shifts for f2 always greater, than envelope-induced shifts observed in isolated sinusoidal tones of comparable frequency and envelope decay rate. Pitch-shift functions for virtual pitch are similar in magnitude and shape to average pitch-shift functions of the partials, which supports the idea that virtual pitch depends on spectral pitch.  相似文献   

7.
Hearing a mistuned harmonic in an otherwise periodic complex tone   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The ability of a listener to detect a mistuned harmonic in an otherwise periodic tone is representative of the capacity to segregate auditory entities on the basis of steady-state signal cues. By use of a task in which listeners matched the pitch of a mistuned harmonic, this ability has been studied, in order to find dependences on mistuned harmonic number, fundamental frequency, signal level, and signal duration. The results considerably augment the data previously obtained from discrimination experiments and from experiments in which listeners counted apparent sources. Although previous work has emphasized the role of spectral resolution in the segregation process, the present work suggests that neural synchrony is an important consideration; our data show that listeners lose the ability to segregate mistuned harmonics at high frequencies where synchronous neural firing vanishes. The functional form of this loss is insensitive to the spacing of the harmonics. The matching experiment also permits the measurement of the pitches of mistuned harmonics. The data exhibit shifts of a form that argues against models of pitch shifts that are based entirely upon partial masking.  相似文献   

8.
When a partial of a periodic complex is mistuned, its change in pitch is greater than expected. Two experiments examined whether these partial-pitch shifts are related to the computation of global pitch. In experiment 1, stimuli were either harmonic or frequency-shifted (25% of F0) complexes. One partial was mistuned by +/- 4% and played with leading and lagging portions of 500 ms each, relative to the other components (1 s), in both monaural and dichotic contexts. Subjects indicated whether the mistuned partial was higher or lower in pitch when concurrent with the other components. Responses were positively correlated with the direction of mistuning in all conditions. In experiment 2, stimuli from each condition were compared with synchronous equivalents. Subjects matched a pure tone to the pitch of the mistuned partial (component 4). The results showed that partial-pitch shifts are not reduced in size by asynchrony. Similar asynchronies are known to produce a near-exclusion of a mistuned partial from the global-pitch computation. This mismatch indicates that global and partial pitch are derived from different processes. The similarity of the partial-pitch shifts observed for harmonic and frequency-shifted stimuli suggests that they arise from a grouping mechanism that is sensitive to spectral regularity.  相似文献   

9.
The experiment compared the pitches of complex tones consisting of unresolved harmonics. The fundamental frequency (F0) of the tones was 250 Hz and the harmonics were bandpass filtered between 5500 and 7500 Hz. Two 20-ms complex-tone bursts were presented, separated by a brief gap. The gap was an integer number of periods of the waveform: 0, 4, or 8 ms. The envelope phase of the second tone burst was shifted, such that the interpulse interval (IPI) across the gap was reduced or increased by 0.25 or 0.75 periods (1 or 3 ms). A "no shift" control was also included, where the IPI was held at an integer number of periods. Pitch matches were obtained by varying the F0 of a comparison tone with the same temporal parameters as the standard but without the shift. Relative to the no-shift control, the variations in IPI produced substantial pitch shifts when there was no gap between the bursts, but little effect was seen for gaps of 4 or 8 ms. However, for some conditions with the same IPI in the shifted interval, an increase in the IPI of the comparison interval from 4 to 8 ms (gap increased from 0 to 4 ms) changed the pitch match. The presence of a pitch shift suggests that the pitch mechanism is integrating information across the two tone bursts. It is argued that the results are consistent with a pitch mechanism employing a long integration time for continuous stimuli that is reset in response to temporal discontinuities. For a 250-Hz F0, an 8-ms IPI may be sufficient for resetting. Pitch models based on a spectral analysis of the simulated neural spike train, on an autocorrelation of the spike train, and on the mean rate of pitch pulses, all failed to account for the observed pitch matches.  相似文献   

10.
The ability to separate simultaneous auditory objects is crucial to infant auditory development. Music in particular relies on the ability to separate musical notes, chords, and melodic lines. Little research addresses how infants process simultaneous sounds. The present study used a conditioned head-turn procedure to examine whether 6-month-old infants are able to discriminate a complex tone (240 Hz, 500 ms, six harmonics in random phase with a 6 dB roll-off per octave) from a version with the third harmonic mistuned. Adults perceive such stimuli as containing two auditory objects, one with the pitch of the mistuned harmonic and the other with pitch corresponding to the fundamental of the complex tone. Adult thresholds were between 1% and 2% mistuning. Infants performed above chance levels for 8%, 6%, and 4% mistunings, with no significant difference between conditions. However, performance was not significantly different from chance for 2% mistuning and significantly worse for 2% compared to all larger mistunings. These results indicate that 6-month-old infants are sensitive to violations of harmonic structure and suggest that they are able to separate two simultaneously sounding objects.  相似文献   

11.
These experiments address the following issues. (1) When two complex tones contain different harmonics, do the differences in timbre between them impair the ability to discriminate the pitches of the tones? (2) When two complex tones have only a single component in common, and that component is the most discriminable component in each tone, is the frequency discrimination of the component affected by differences in residue pitch between the two tones? (3) How good is the pitch discrimination of complex tones with no common components when each tone contains multiple harmonics, so as to avoid ambiguity of pitch? (4) Is the pitch discrimination of complex tones with common harmonics impaired by shifting the component frequencies to nonharmonic values? In all experiments, frequency difference limens (DLCs) were measured for multiple-component complex tones, using an adaptive two-interval, two-alternative, forced-choice task. Three highly trained subjects were used. The main conclusions are as follows. (1) When two tones have the first six harmonics in common, DLCs are larger when the upper harmonics are different than when the upper harmonics are in common or are absent. It appears that differences in timbre impair DLCs. (2) Discrimination of the frequency of a single common partial in two complex tones is worse when the two tones have different residue pitches than when they have the same residue pitch. (3) DLCs for complex tones with no common harmonics are generally larger than those for complex tones with common harmonics. For the former, large individual differences occur, probably because subjects are affected differently by differences in timbre. (4) DLCs for harmonic complex tones are smaller than DLCs for complex tones in which the components are mistuned from harmonic values. This can probably be attributed to the less distinct residue pitch of the inharmonic complexes, rather than to reduced discriminability of partials. Overall, the results support the idea that DLCs for complex tones with common harmonics depend on residue pitch comparisons, rather than on comparisons of the pitches of partials.  相似文献   

12.
The discrimination of the fundamental frequency (fo) of pairs of complex tones with no common harmonics is worse than the discrimination of fo for tones with all harmonics in common. These experiments were conducted to assess whether this effect is a result of pitch shifts between pairs of tones without common harmonics or whether it reflects influences of spectral differences (timbre) on the accuracy of pitch perception. In experiment 1, pitch matches were obtained between sounds drawn from the following types: (1) pure tones (P) with frequencies 100, 200, or 400 Hz; (2) a multiple-component complex tone, designated A, with harmonics 3, 4, 8, 9, 10, 14, 15, and fo = 100, 200, or 400 Hz; (3) A multiple-component complex tone, designated B, with harmonics 5, 6, 7, 11, 12, 13, 16, and with fo = 100, 200 or 400 Hz. The following matches were made; A vs A, B vs B, A vs P, B vs P and P vs P. Pitch shifts were found between the pure tones and the complex tones (A vs P and B vs P), but not between the A and B tones (A vs B). However, the variability of the A vs B matches was significantly greater than that of the A vs A or B vs B matches. Also, the variability of the A vs P and B vs P matches was greater than that for the A vs B matches. In a second experiment, frequency difference limens (DLCs) were measured for the A vs A, B vs B, and A vs B pairs of sounds. The DLCs were larger for the A vs B pair than for A vs A or B vs B. The results suggest that the poor frequency discrimination of tones with no common harmonics does not result from pitch shifts between the tones. Rather, it seems that spectral differences between tones interfere with judgements of their relative pitch.  相似文献   

13.
Learning to perceive pitch differences   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
This paper reports two experiments concerning the stimulus specificity of pitch discrimination learning. In experiment 1, listeners were initially trained, during ten sessions (about 11,000 trials), to discriminate a monaural pure tone of 3000 Hz from ipsilateral pure tones with slightly different frequencies. The resulting perceptual learning (improvement in discrimination thresholds) appeared to be frequency-specific since, in subsequent sessions, new learning was observed when the 3000-Hz standard tone was replaced by a standard tone of 1200 Hz, or 6500 Hz. By contrast, a subsequent presentation of the initial tones to the contralateral ear showed that the initial learning was not, or was only weakly, ear-specific. In experiment 2, training in pitch discrimination was initially provided using complex tones that consisted of harmonics 3-7 of a missing fundamental (near 100 Hz for some listeners, 500 Hz for others). Subsequently, the standard complex was replaced by a standard pure tone with a frequency which could be either equal to the standard complex's missing fundamental or remote from it. In the former case, the two standard stimuli were matched in pitch. However, this perceptual relationship did not appear to favor the transfer of learning. Therefore, the results indicated that pitch discrimination learning is, at least to some extent, timbre-specific, and cannot be viewed as a reduction of an internal noise which would affect directly the output of a neural device extracting pitch from both pure tones and complex tones including low-rank harmonics.  相似文献   

14.
It is unclear whether the perceptual segregation of a mistuned harmonic from a periodic complex tone depends specifically on harmonic relations between the other components. A procedure used previously for harmonic complexes [W. M. Hartmann et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 88, 1712-1724 (1990)] was adapted and extended to regular inharmonic complexes. On each trial, subjects heard a 12-component complex followed by a pure tone in a continuous loop. In experiment 1, a mistuning of +/- 4% was applied to one of the components 2-11. The complex was either harmonic, frequency shifted, or spectrally stretched. Subjects adjusted the pure tone to match the pitch of the mistuned component. Near matches were taken to indicate segregation, and were almost as frequent in the inharmonic conditions as in the harmonic case. Also, small but consistent mismatches, pitch shifts, were found in all conditions. These were similar in direction and size to earlier findings for harmonic complexes. Using a range of mistunings, experiment 2 showed that the segregation of components from regular inharmonic complexes could be sensitive to mistunings of 1.5% or less. These findings are consistent with the proposal that aspects of spectral regularity other than harmonic relations can also influence auditory grouping.  相似文献   

15.
Thresholds for the discrimination of fundamental frequency (FODLs) and frequency difference limens (FDLs) for individual partials within a complex tone (F0=250 Hz, harmonics 1-7) were measured for stimulus durations of 200, 50, and 16 ms. The FDLs increased with decreasing duration. Although the results differed across subjects, the effect of duration generally decreased as the harmonic number increased from 1 to 4, then increased as the harmonic number increased to 6, and finally decreased for the seventh harmonic. For each duration, FODLs were smaller than the smallest FDL for any individual harmonic, indicating that information is combined across harmonics in the discrimination of FO. FODLs predicted from the FDLs corresponded well with observed FODLs for the 200- and 16-ms durations but were significantly larger than observed FODLs for the 50-ms duration. A supplementary pitch-matching experiment using two subjects indicated that the contribution of the seventh harmonic to the pitch of the 16-ms complex tone was smaller than would be predicted from the FDL for that harmonic. The results are consistent with the idea that the dominant region shifts upward with decreasing duration, but that the weight assigned to individual harmonics is not always adjusted in an optimal way.  相似文献   

16.
Perception of the missing fundamental in nonhuman primates   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
In preparation for neurophysiological experiments aimed at mechanisms of pitch perception, four rhesus monkeys were trained to press a button when the fundamental frequencies (missing or present) of two complex tones in a tone pair matched. Both tones were based on a five-component harmonic series. Zero to three of the lowest components could be missing in the first tone, while the second (comparison) tone contained all five harmonics. The range of fundamentals tested varied from 200 to 600 Hz. Three monkeys learned to match tones missing their fundamentals to comparison harmonic complexes with the same pitch, whereas the fourth monkey required the physical presence of the fundamental. Consideration of several cues available to the monkeys suggests that the animals could perceive the missing fundamental.  相似文献   

17.
Psychoacoustic experiments were performed to measure the pitch-shift effects of pure and complex tones resulting from the addition of a masking noise to the tonal stimuli. Harmonic residue tones with either two or three harmonics and a fundamental frequency of 200 Hz were chosen as test tones. The pitch shifts of virtual and spectral pitches of the residue tones were measured as a function of the intensity of a low-pass noise with 600-Hz cutoff frequency. The SPL of this noise varied between 30 and 70 dB. In another experiment, the pitch shifts of single pure tones corresponding to the frequencies and SPLs of the harmonics of the residue tones were measured using the same masking noise. The results from five subjects for the harmonic residue tones show only a weak dependence of pitch shift on masking noise intensity. This dependence exists for both spectral and virtual pitches. In the case of single pure tones, pitch shift depends more distinctly on noise intensity. Pitch shifts of up to 5% were found in the range of noise intensity investigated. The magnitude of pitch shift shows pronounced interindividual differences, but the direction of the shift effect is always the same. In all cases pitch increases with higher masking noise levels.  相似文献   

18.
These experiments measure the ability to detect a change in the relative phase of a single component in a harmonic complex tone. Complex tones containing the first 20 harmonics of 50, 100, or 200 Hz, all at equal amplitude, were used. All of the harmonics except one started in cosine phase. The remaining harmonic started in cosine phase, but was shifted in phase half-way through either the first or the second of the two stimuli comprising a trial. The subject had to identify the stimulus containing the phase-shifted component. For normally hearing subjects tested at a level of 70 dB SPL per component, thresholds for detecting the phase shift [i.e., phase difference limens (DLs)] were smallest (2 degrees-4 degrees) for harmonics above the eighth and for the lowest fundamental frequency (F0). Changes in phase were not detectable for harmonic numbers below three or four at the lowest F0 and below 5-13 at the highest F0. The DLs increased slightly for the highest harmonics in the complexes. The DLs increased markedly with decreasing level, except for the highest harmonic, where only a small effect of level was found. Subjects reported that the phase-shifted harmonic appeared to "pop out" and was heard with a pure-tone quality. A pitch-matching experiment demonstrated that the pitch of this tone corresponded to the frequency of the phase-shifted component. For the highest harmonic, the phase shift was associated with a downward shift of the edge pitch heard in the reference (all cosine phase) stimulus. When the phases of the components in the reference stimulus were randomized, phase DLs were much higher (and often impossible to measure), the pop-out phenomenon was not observed, and no edge pitch was heard. Subjects with unilateral cochlear hearing impairment generally showed poorer phase sensitivity in their impaired than in their normal ears, when the two ears were compared at equal sound-pressure levels. However, at comparable sensation levels, the impaired ears sometimes showed lower phase DLs. The results are explained by considering the waveforms that would occur at the outputs of the auditory filters in response to these stimuli.  相似文献   

19.
Mistuning one partial of a complex harmonic tone makes that partial easier to hear as a tone separate from the complex. At the same time, two pitch shifts may be observed. First, the low pitch of the complex is shifted in the direction of the mistuning, as if it were "pulled" by the partial. Second, the mistuning of the partial is perceptually exaggerated, as if the pitch of the partial were "pushed" away from the harmonic series defined by the complex. This paper shows how the latter effect can emerge within a hypothetical neural circuit. The circuit involves a gating neuron fed by three pathways, one direct and excitatory and the other two delayed and inhibitory. The neuron responds to any excitatory input spike unless it is accompanied by an inhibitory input spike on either delayed input, thus acting as a kind of "anticoincidence counter." The first delay is fixed and tuned to the period of the background harmonic complex. Its purpose is to weaken correlates of in-tune components and allow the mistuned partial to stand out. The second delay is variable and used to estimate the period of the mistuned partial, by searching for a minimum output as a function of delay. With an appropriate choice of parameters, the estimate is subject to shifts that are of the same sign as the mistuning and that peak at about 4% mistuning and decrease beyond, as observed experimentally.  相似文献   

20.
Thresholds for the detection of harmonic complex tones in noise were measured as a function of masker level. The rms level of the masker ranged from 40 to 70 dB SPL in 10-dB steps. The tones had a fundamental frequency (F0) of 62.5 or 250 Hz, and components were added in either cosine or random phase. The complex tones and the noise were bandpass filtered into the same frequency region, from the tenth harmonic up to 5 kHz. In a different condition, the roles of masker and signal were reversed, keeping all other parameters the same; subjects had to detect the noise in the presence of a harmonic tone masker. In both conditions, the masker was either gated synchronously with the 700-ms signal, or it started 400 ms before and stopped 200 ms after the signal. The results showed a large asymmetry in the effectiveness of masking between the tones and noise. Even though signal and masker had the same bandwidth, the noise was a more effective masker than the complex tone. The degree of asymmetry depended on F0, component phase, and the level of the masker. The maximum difference between masked thresholds for tone and noise was about 28 dB; this occurred when the F0 was 62.5 Hz, the components were in cosine phase, and the masker level was 70 dB SPL. In most conditions, the growth-of-masking functions had slopes close to 1 (on a dB versus dB scale). However, for the cosine-phase tone masker with an F0 of 62.5 Hz, a 10-dB increase in masker level led to an increase in masked threshold of the noise of only 3.7 dB, on average. We suggest that the results for this condition are strongly affected by the active mechanism in the cochlea.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号