首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 10 毫秒
1.
Several listening conditions show that energy remote from a target frequency can deleteriously affect sensitivity. One interpretation of such results entails a wideband analysis involving a wide predetection filter. The present study tested the hypothesis that both temporal gap detection and overshoot results are consistent with a wideband analysis, as contrasted with statistical combination of information across independent channels. For gap detection, stimuli were random or comodulated 50-Hz-wide noise bands centered on 1000, 1932, 3569, and 6437 Hz. For overshoot, the masker was an 8-kHz low-pass filtered noise, with 5-ms tone bursts presented at the same center frequencies used for gap detection. Signals were presented with either 0- or 250-ms delay after masker onset. In each paradigm, the target was introduced at only one frequency or at all four frequencies. Results from gap detection conditions did not favor a wideband analysis interpretation: Results in the random condition were consistent with an optimal combination of cues across frequency. An across-channel interference effect was also evident when only one of the four bands contained the gap. Although results from the overshoot conditions were consistent with a wideband analysis interpretation, they were more parsimoniously accounted for in terms of statistical combination of information.  相似文献   

2.
Gap detection thresholds were measured by forced-choice procedure for conditions where the duration of a silent gap was varied adaptively between pairs of sinusoidal markers of the same or different frequency. Frequencies of the first sinusoid in a pair of markers ranged from F1 = 500 to 4000 Hz. Second-sinusoid marker frequencies F2 included F1 = F2, and usually frequencies 2%, 5%, 24%, and 50% higher than F1. In preliminary studies the role of presentation level (E/N0) on gap detection was considered. Preliminary data revealed confounding extraneous factors arising from gating transients and from overall stimulus (i.e., markers + gap) and/or masker duration cues. In the main experiments, the contributions of these extraneous cues were evaluated with experimental designs aimed at identifying and minimizing the confounding roles of these cues in gap detection. For conditions where extraneous gating transient cues were minimized (by presenting the sinusoidal markers in a continuous noise masker with random onset phase for the second sinusoid in every pair of markers) and overall stimulus duration cues were diminished (by randomizing the duration of each marker independently), gap detection thresholds increased from 5 to 90 ms as the frequency separation between F1 and F2 was increased by half an octave. When the gap detection thresholds were treated as filter attenuation values by normalizing and converting the data into decibels, the data were closely fit by the roex filter model. On average, the listeners' performances were modeled well by a constant-percentage (7%) bandwidth filter centered on F1.  相似文献   

3.
We present what we believe to be the first experimental demonstration of anomalous spectral behavior such as spectral shifts and spectral switches due to temporal correlation around the intensity minima in a white-light interferometer. Unusual behavior in the number of spectral fringes, measured within the source bandwidth, as a function of path delay between the interfering beams is also reported. Experimental observations match well with the spectra calculated by using the interference law in the spectral domain.  相似文献   

4.
The purpose of this report is to present new data that provide a novel perspective on temporal masking, different from that found in the classical auditory literature on this topic. Specifically, measurement conditions are presented that minimize rather than maximize temporal spread of masking for a gated (200-ms) narrow-band (405-Hz-wide) noise masker logarithmically centered at 2500 Hz. Masked detection thresholds were measured for brief sinusoids in a two-interval, forced-choice (21FC) task. Detection was measured at each of 43 temporal positions within the signal observation interval for the sinusoidal signal presented either preceding, during, or following the gating of the masker, which was centered temporally within each 500-ms observation interval. Results are presented for three listeners; first, for detection of a 1900-Hz signal across a range of masker component levels (0-70 dB SPL) and, second, for masked detection as a function of signal frequency (fs = 500-5000 Hz) for a fixed masker component level (40 dB SPL). For signals presented off-frequency from the masker, and at low-to-moderate masker levels, the resulting temporal masking functions are characterized by sharp temporal edges. The sharpness of the edges is accentuated by complex patterns of temporal overshoot and undershoot, corresponding with diminished and enhanced detection, respectively, at both masker onset and offset. This information about the onset and offset timing of the gated masker is faithfully represented in the temporal masking functions over the full decade range of signal frequencies (except for fs=2500 Hz presented at the center frequency of the masker). The precise representation of the timing information is remarkable considering that the temporal envelope characteristics of the gated masker are evident in the remote masking response at least two octaves below the frequencies of the masker at a cochlear place where little or no masker activity would be expected. This general enhancement of the temporal edges of the masking response is reminiscent of spectral edge enhancement by lateral suppression/inhibition.  相似文献   

5.
Results of experiments on the detection of silent intervals, or gaps, in broadband noise are reported for normal-hearing listeners. In some preliminary experiments, a gap threshold of about 2 ms was measured. This value was independent of the duration of the noise burst, variation of the noise level on each presentation, or the temporal position of the gap within the noise burst. In the main experiments, the thresholds for partial decrements in the noise waveform as well as brief increments were determined. As predicted by a model that assumes a single fixed peak-to-valley detection ratio, thresholds for increments are slightly higher than thresholds for decrements when the signal is measured as the change in rms noise level. A first-order model describes the temporal properties of the auditory system as a low-pass filter with a 7- to 8-ms time constant. Temporal modulation transfer functions were determined for the same subjects, and the estimated temporal parameters agreed well with those estimated from the gap detection data. More detailed modeling was carried out by simulating Viemeister's three-stage temporal model. Simulations, using an initial stage bandwidth of 4000 Hz and a 3-ms time constant for the low-pass filter, generate data that are very similar to those obtained from human subjects in both modulation and gap detection.  相似文献   

6.
George G. Szpiro 《Physica A》2007,384(2):291-296
Prime numbers seem to be distributed somewhat randomly throughout the sequence of natural numbers. It is known, however, that gaps between consecutive primes exhibit a preponderance towards 6 and its multiples, and recently it was shown that the gaps between the gaps also exhibit a peculiar pattern. In this paper higher-order gaps are analyzed using Fourier decomposition. The resulting power spectra reveal additional patterns in the prime number sequence.  相似文献   

7.
Talkers change the way they speak in noisy conditions. For energetic maskers, speech production changes are relatively well-understood, but less is known about how informational maskers such as competing speech affect speech production. The current study examines the effect of energetic and informational maskers on speech production by talkers speaking alone or in pairs. Talkers produced speech in quiet and in backgrounds of speech-shaped noise, speech-modulated noise, and competing speech. Relative to quiet, speech output level and fundamental frequency increased and spectral tilt flattened in proportion to the energetic masking capacity of the background. In response to modulated backgrounds, talkers were able to reduce substantially the degree of temporal overlap with the noise, with greater reduction for the competing speech background. Reduction in foreground-background overlap can be expected to lead to a release from both energetic and informational masking for listeners. Passive changes in speech rate, mean pause length or pause distribution cannot explain the overlap reduction, which appears instead to result from a purposeful process of listening while speaking. Talkers appear to monitor the background and exploit upcoming pauses, a strategy which is particularly effective for backgrounds containing intelligible speech.  相似文献   

8.
9.
We report a detailed analysis of experimental parameters and fundamental mechanisms contributing to the strong nonlinear scattering of femtosecond and short picosecond laser pulses (50–6000 fs) at focusing in ambient air and protecting gases. The experimental conditions under consideration are typical for a variety of applications: micromachining of metals and dielectrics, high power laser exposure of targets. Such scattering, being the most noticeable manifestation of the complex phenomenon of conical emission in the focusing geometry, puts fundamental limitations to the incident power of precision laser targeting, restricting in this way potential productivity of micromachining in this pulse width range. The phenomena analyzed here are: ionization, the optical Kerr effect, the distortion of spectra via self-phase modulation, and the refractive ability of microplasma, namely the scattering. Characteristics of the last were investigated with respect to the incident wavelength and pulse width, for focusing of Gaussian and flat-top beams of different diameters. The obtained extensive data allowed us to trace the clue trends and relationships of this phenomenon and to find several ways to reduce or even eliminate the scattering in a broad range of the incident energies.  相似文献   

10.
The COSS analysis [B. G. Berg, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 86, 1743-1746 (1989)] was used to estimate spectral and temporal weights of a three-component, amplitude-modulated stimulus in a spectral-shape discrimination task. In all experiments, the task of the observer was to detect an increment in the level of the center component. A spectral-temporal weight quantifies the relative influence of a spectral component on the decisions of an observer during a specified segment of the total stimulus duration. In the first two experiments, the signal was added to all three temporal segments of the center component. The ideal weights for each component should be the same across temporal segments. Spectral-temporal weights were obtained for four conditions with different stimulus durations. In general, the estimated weights for each component were not equal at different temporal segments. In the third experiment, the signal was added to only one of three segments of the center component. Ideally, weight patterns should have changed when the temporal position of the signal segment was altered. Two stimulus durations, 300 and 15 ms, were used. For the 300-ms condition, the signal was added to only the end segment, and for all three observers the weight patterns are different from that obtained in experiment 1 with the signal added to all segments. For the 15-ms conditions, altering the signal position changed the estimated weights for only one observer.  相似文献   

11.
A two-alternative forced-choice task was used to measure psychometric functions for the detection of temporal gaps in a 1-kHz, 400-ms sinusoidal signal. The signal always started and finished at a positive-going zero crossing, and the gap duration was varied from 0.5 to 6.0 ms in 0.5-ms steps. The signal level was 80 dB SPL, and a spectrally shaped noise was used to mask splatter associated with the abrupt onset and offset of the signal. Two subjects with normal hearing, two subjects with unilateral cochlear hearing loss, and two subjects with bilateral cochlear hearing loss were tested. The impaired ears had confirmed reductions in frequency selectivity at 1 kHz. For the normal ears, the psychometric functions were nonmonotonic, showing minima for gap durations corresponding to integer multiples of the signal period (n ms, where n is a positive integer) and maxima for durations corresponding to (n - 0.5) ms. For the impaired ears, the psychometric functions showed only small (nonsignificant) nonmonotonicities. Performance overall was slightly worse for the impaired than for the normal ears. The main features of the results could be accounted for using a model consisting of a bandpass filter (the auditory filter), a square-law device, and a sliding temporal integrator. Consistent with the data, the model demonstrates that, although a broader auditory filter has a faster transient response, this does not necessarily lead to improved performance in a gap detection task. The model also indicates that gap thresholds do not provide a direct measure of temporal resolution, since they depend at least partly on intensity resolution.  相似文献   

12.
Cochlear implant users receive limited spectral and temporal information. Their speech recognition deteriorates dramatically in noise. The aim of the present study was to determine the relative contributions of spectral and temporal cues to speech recognition in noise. Spectral information was manipulated by varying the number of channels from 2 to 32 in a noise-excited vocoder. Temporal information was manipulated by varying the low-pass cutoff frequency of the envelope extractor from 1 to 512 Hz. Ten normal-hearing, native speakers of English participated in tests of phoneme recognition using vocoder processed consonants and vowels under three conditions (quiet, and +6 and 0 dB signal-to-noise ratios). The number of channels required for vowel-recognition performance to plateau increased from 12 in quiet to 16-24 in the two noise conditions. However, for consonant recognition, no further improvement in performance was evident when the number of channels was > or =12 in any of the three conditions. The contribution of temporal cues for phoneme recognition showed a similar pattern in both quiet and noise conditions. Similar to the quiet conditions, there was a trade-off between temporal and spectral cues for phoneme recognition in noise.  相似文献   

13.
The concept of temporal superresolution is applied to optical few-cycle laser pulses for the first time to our knowledge. Pulse durations of as little as to 3.7 fs, well below the Fourier limit, are achieved by pulse shaping of an octave-spanning Ti:sapphire oscillator spectrum. Our prism-based pulse shaper also enables us to generate a manifold of well-controlled pulse sequences that are important for coherent control applications on a femtosecond time scale.  相似文献   

14.
In an effort to provide a unifying framework for understanding monaural and binaural processing of intensity differences, an experiment was performed to assess whether temporal weighting functions estimated in two-interval monaural intensity-discrimination tasks could account for data in single-interval interaural intensity-discrimination tasks. In both tasks, stimuli consisted of a 50-ms burst of noise with a 5-ms probe segment at temporal positions ranging between the onset and offset of the overall stimulus. During the probe segment, one monaural interval or binaural channel of each trial contained an intensity increment and the other contained a decrement. Listeners were instructed to choose the interval/channel containing the increment. The pattern of monaural thresholds was roughly symmetrical (an inverted U) across temporal position of the probe but interaural thresholds were substantially higher for a brief time interval following stimulus onset. A two-sided exponential temporal window fit to the monaural data accounted for the interaural data well when combined with a post-onset-weighting function that described greatest weighting of binaural information at stimulus onset. A second experiment showed that the specific procedure used in measuring fringed interaural-intensity-difference-discrimination thresholds affects thresholds as a function of fringe duration and influences the form of the best-fitting post-onset-weighting function.  相似文献   

15.
Two experiments measured human sensitivity to temporal jitter in 25-click trains with inter-click intervals (ICIs) between 5 and 100 ms. In a naturalistic experiment using wideband clicks, jitter thresholds were a nonmonotonic function of ICI, peaking for ICIs near 40-60 ms. In a subsequent experiment, clicks were high-passed and presented against a low-frequency noise masker. Jitter threshold vs ICI functions lost the positive slope over short ICIs but retained the negative slope at long ICIs. The same behavior was seen in click rate discrimination tasks. Different processes mediate regularity analysis for click trains with ICIs above and below 40-60 ms.  相似文献   

16.
This paper describes a detailed study of recovery from forward masking in six users of the Nucleus-22 cochlear implant with a range of performance in speech-recognition tests. Recovery from a 300-ms-long pulse train presented at 1000 pps was found to be fastest in the poorer performers. The shape of the recovery function was found to be most strongly influenced by masker duration, suggesting that temporal integration plays a prominent role in recovery from forward masking. The recovery functions are reasonably well described by a sum of two exponentially decaying processes. Their relative weights depend on the amount of temporal integration occurring during the masker, and show strong intersubject variability. Nonmonotonicities sometimes observed in the recovery functions may be accounted for by considering the influence of neural adaptation.  相似文献   

17.
The concern of this review is brain theory or more specifically, in its first part, a model of the cerebral cortex and the way it: (a) interacts with subcortical regions like the thalamus and the hippocampus to provide higher-level-brain functions that underlie cognition and intelligence, (b) handles and represents dynamical sensory patterns imposed by a constantly changing environment, (c) copes with the enormous number of such patterns encountered in a lifetime by means of dynamic memory that offers an immense number of stimulus-specific attractors for input patterns (stimuli) to select from, (d) selects an attractor through a process of “conjugation” of the input pattern with the dynamics of the thalamo–cortical loop, (e) distinguishes between redundant (structured) and non-redundant (random) inputs that are void of information, (f) can do categorical perception when there is access to vast associative memory laid out in the association cortex with the help of the hippocampus, and (g) makes use of “computation” at the edge of chaos and information driven annealing to achieve all this. Other features and implications of the concepts presented for the design of computational algorithms and machines with brain-like intelligence are also discussed. The material and results presented suggest, that a Parametrically Coupled Logistic Map network (PCLMN) is a minimal model of the thalamo–cortical complex and that marrying such a network to a suitable associative memory with re-entry or feedback forms a useful, albeit, abstract model of a cortical module of the brain that could facilitate building a simple artificial brain.

In the second part of the review, the results of numerical simulations and drawn conclusions in the first part are linked to the most directly relevant works and views of other workers. What emerges is a picture of brain dynamics on the mesoscopic and macroscopic scales that gives a glimpse of the nature of the long sought after brain code underlying intelligence and other higher level brain functions.  相似文献   


18.
The generation mechanisms of real world networks have been described using multiple models. The mathematical features of these models are usually extrapolated from statistical properties of a snapshot of these networks. We here propose an alternative method based on direct measurement of a sequence of consecutive snapshots to uncover the dynamics underlying real world generation. We assume that the probability of adding a node or an edge depends only on local features surrounding the newly added node/edge, and directly measure the contribution of these features to the node/edge addition probability. These measurements are performed using newly defined N-node local structures. Each N-node local structure represents the configuration of edges surrounding a newly added edge. The N-node local structure measurements reproduce for some networks the now classical addition of edges between high degree node mechanisms. It also provides quantitative estimates of more complex mechanisms driving other networks’ evolution, such as the effect of common first and second neighbors. This new methodology reveals the relative importance of different generation mechanisms. We show, for example, that the main mechanism driving hyperlink addition between two websites is the existence of a third website linking to both the source and the target of the new hyperlink.  相似文献   

19.
When a system composed of dissolved laser-polarized xenon with negative spin temperature is put inside a high field NMR magnet, a series of spontaneous maser emissions can be observed. We report here their spectral and temporal features using a processing model derived from the solution of the Bloch equations in the presence of radiation damping. We show, in particular, that by combining Fourier transformation and squared modulus, a parameter allowing the characterization of the burst of transverse magnetization can be determined. This parameter is shown to be correlated with the radiated energy. Moreover, this processing clearly reveals features which can probably be assigned to effects resulting from distant dipolar fields. Finally, the analysis of the experimental data reveals an unexpected behavior of the 129Xe transverse self-relaxation.  相似文献   

20.
Systems showing stochastic resonance (SR) or coherent resonance (CR) share some features, in particular, the nearly periodic character of the signal. We show that in spite of this resemblance the different underlying dynamics can be detected in experimental data by studying the histogram of the inter-spikes times and some statistical properties like two-time correlation functions. We discuss its possible relevance for climate modeling.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号