首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Gap detection thresholds were measured in patients with the Nucleus and Symbion cochlear implants as a function of several current waveform parameters. Detection of gaps in an electrical sinusoidal stimulus or in a train of biphasic pulses by implanted patients was similar to detection of gaps in comparable acoustic stimuli by normal listeners. Threshold gaps were 20-50 ms for low-level stimuli and improved with stimulus level to 2-5 ms for high-level stimuli. Gap detection performance was not affected by the electrode position in the cochlea or by the distance between stimulating electrodes. The data from most patients were well fitted by a trading relation between the duration of the gap and the square of stimulus intensity, indicating energy detection. The similarity of gap thresholds for normal subjects and implant patients suggests that many details of the peripheral neural activity are probably not important for this task, and that there is no retrocochlear loss of auditory temporal resolution with sensorineural hearing loss.  相似文献   

2.
3.
These experiments measure the ability to detect a change in the relative phase of a single component in a harmonic complex tone. Complex tones containing the first 20 harmonics of 50, 100, or 200 Hz, all at equal amplitude, were used. All of the harmonics except one started in cosine phase. The remaining harmonic started in cosine phase, but was shifted in phase half-way through either the first or the second of the two stimuli comprising a trial. The subject had to identify the stimulus containing the phase-shifted component. For normally hearing subjects tested at a level of 70 dB SPL per component, thresholds for detecting the phase shift [i.e., phase difference limens (DLs)] were smallest (2 degrees-4 degrees) for harmonics above the eighth and for the lowest fundamental frequency (F0). Changes in phase were not detectable for harmonic numbers below three or four at the lowest F0 and below 5-13 at the highest F0. The DLs increased slightly for the highest harmonics in the complexes. The DLs increased markedly with decreasing level, except for the highest harmonic, where only a small effect of level was found. Subjects reported that the phase-shifted harmonic appeared to "pop out" and was heard with a pure-tone quality. A pitch-matching experiment demonstrated that the pitch of this tone corresponded to the frequency of the phase-shifted component. For the highest harmonic, the phase shift was associated with a downward shift of the edge pitch heard in the reference (all cosine phase) stimulus. When the phases of the components in the reference stimulus were randomized, phase DLs were much higher (and often impossible to measure), the pop-out phenomenon was not observed, and no edge pitch was heard. Subjects with unilateral cochlear hearing impairment generally showed poorer phase sensitivity in their impaired than in their normal ears, when the two ears were compared at equal sound-pressure levels. However, at comparable sensation levels, the impaired ears sometimes showed lower phase DLs. The results are explained by considering the waveforms that would occur at the outputs of the auditory filters in response to these stimuli.  相似文献   

4.
The growth of distortion product otoacoustic emission (DPOAE) suppression follows a systematic, frequency-dependent pattern. The pattern is consistent with direct measures of basilar-membrane response growth, psychoacoustic measures of masking growth, and measures of neural rate growth. This pattern has its basis in the recognized nonlinear properties of basilar-membrane motion and, as such, the DPOAE suppression growth paradigm can be applied to human neonates to study the maturation of cochlear nonlinearity. The objective of this experiment was to investigate the maturation of human cochlear nonlinearity and define the time course for this maturational process. Normal-hearing adults, children, term-born neonates, and premature neonates, plus a small number of children with sensorineural hearing loss, were included in this experiment. DPOAE suppression growth was measured at two f2 frequencies (1500 and 6000 Hz) and three primary tone levels (55-45, 65-55, and 75-65 dB SPL). Slope of DPOAE suppression growth, as well as an asymmetry ratio (to compare slope for suppressor tones below and above f2 frequency), were generated. Suppression threshold was also measured in all subjects. Findings indicate that both term-born neonates and premature neonates who have attained term-like age, show non-adult-like DPOAE suppression growth for low-frequency suppressor tones. These age effects are most evident at f2 = 6000 Hz. In neonates, suppression growth is shallower and suppression thresholds are elevated for suppressor tones lower in frequency than f2. Additionally, the asymmetry ratio is smaller in neonates, indicating that the typical frequency-dependent pattern of suppression growth is not present. These findings suggest that an immaturity of cochlear nonlinearity persists into the first months of postnatal life. DPOAE suppression growth examined for a small group of hearing-impaired children also showed abnormalities.  相似文献   

5.
Transient-evoked otoacoustic emissions (TEOAE) are responses generated within the inner ear in response to acoustic stimuli and are indicative of normal cochlear function. They are commonly acquired by averaging post-stimulus acoustic responses recorded near the eardrum in response to brief stimuli such as clicks or tone pips. In this study a new long duration stimulus consisting of a frequency swept tone is introduced for the acquisition of TEOAEs. Like stimulus frequency generated OAEs, swept-tone responses contain embedded OAEs. With swept-tone analysis, OAEs can be recovered by convolving it with a time reversed swept-tone signal resulting in time-compression. In addition, higher order nonlinear OAE responses were removed from the linear TEOAE. The results show comparable phase and time-frequency properties between the click and swept-tone evoked OAEs. Swept-tone acquisition of TEOAEs has beneficial noise properties, improving the signal to noise ratio by 6 dB compared to click evoked responses thus offering testing time savings. Additionally, swept-tone analysis removed synchronized spontaneous OAE activity from the recordings of subjects exhibiting such responses in conventional click TEOAEs. Since swept-tone stimulus consists of a single frequency component at any instantaneous moment, its analysis also provides for direct comparison with stimulus-frequency OAEs and click evoked OAEs.  相似文献   

6.
Threshold and suprathreshold temporal integration (TI) effects were studied in the crossed and uncrossed human acoustic stapedius reflex. Changes in reflex threshold were compared at four stimulus durations and for six threshold response criteria; suprathreshold effects were similarly compared at four stimulus durations and at five stimulus sound-pressure levels. Our results showed that reflex thresholds were significantly lower for the uncrossed condition and for longer duration stimuli. Both effects were dependent on threshold response criteria. Threshold TI measurements in both crossed and uncrossed conditions were equivalent at low criterion levels (0%-4%, p greater than 0.05) but were significantly larger in the crossed condition at higher criterion levels (5%-6%, p less than 0.05). Prominent suprathreshold effects also occurred. The main effect was characterized by significantly larger reflex magnitudes in the uncrossed condition. Duration dependent slope differences in magnitude intensity functions (MIFs) were also observed and were characterized by steeper slopes for longer duration stimuli. Saturation effects and/or intensity-dependent slope decelerations in MIFs were predominant in the uncrossed condition and for stimulus durations exceeding 20 ms. These data provide further quantitative evidence for asymmetric threshold and suprathreshold response properties of the crossed and uncrossed human acoustic stapedius reflex and demonstrate the dependence of these effects on stimuli of different durations.  相似文献   

7.
The acoustic reflex thresholds for broadband noise and 500-, 1000-, and 2000-Hz activating signals were measured in a group of young normal hearing adults and a group of elderly normal hearing subjects. The results indicated that the acoustic reflex thresholds for tonal activating signals in the young subjects were similar to those in the elderly subjects. However, the acoustic stapedius reflex thresholds for broadband noise activating signals is significantly higher in the elderly subjects than in the young. These differences were explained in light of Bredburg's findings [Acta Otolaryngol. Suppl. 236, 1--135 (1968)] regarding degeneration of outer hair cells as a function of aging.  相似文献   

8.
The ability of normally hearing and hearing-impaired subjects to use temporal fine structure information in complex tones was measured. Subjects were required to discriminate a harmonic complex tone from a tone in which all components were shifted upwards by the same amount in Hz, in a three-alternative, forced-choice task. The tones either contained five equal-amplitude components (non-shaped stimuli) or contained many components, but were passed through a fixed bandpass filter to reduce excitation pattern changes (shaped stimuli). Components were centered at nominal harmonic numbers (N) 7, 11, and 18. For the shaped stimuli, hearing-impaired subjects performed much more poorly than normally hearing subjects, with most of the former scoring no better than chance when N=11 or 18, suggesting that they could not access the temporal fine structure information. Performance for the hearing-impaired subjects was significantly improved for the non-shaped stimuli, presumably because they could benefit from spectral cues. It is proposed that normal-hearing subjects can use temporal fine structure information provided the spacing between fine structure peaks is not too small relative to the envelope period, but subjects with moderate cochlear hearing loss make little use of temporal fine structure information for unresolved components.  相似文献   

9.
Although numerous studies have investigated temporal integration of the acoustic-reflex threshold (ART), research is lacking on the effect of age on temporal integration of the ART. Therefore the effect of age on temporal integration of the ART was investigated for a broad-band noise (BBN) activator. Subjects consisted of two groups of adults with normal-hearing sensitivity: one group of 20 young adults (ten males and ten females, ages 18-29 years, with a mean age of 24 years) and one group of 20 older adults (ten males and ten females, ages 59-75 years, with a mean age of 67.5 years). Activating stimulus durations were 12, 25, 50, 100, 200, 300, 500, and 1000 ms. Significant main effects for duration and age were obtained. That is, as the duration increased, the acoustic reflex threshold for BBN decreased. The interactions of duration x age group and duration x hearing level were not significant. The result of pair-wise analysis indicated statistically significant differences between the two age groups at durations of 20 ms and longer. The observed age effect on temporal integration of the ART for the BBN activator is interpreted in relation to senescent changes in the auditory system.  相似文献   

10.
Effects of sound level on auditory cortical activation are seen in neuroimaging data. However, factors such as the cortical response to the intense ambient scanner noise and to the bandwidth of the acoustic stimuli will both confound precise quantification and interpretation of such sound-level effects. The present study used temporally "sparse" imaging to reduce effects of scanner noise. To achieve control for stimulus bandwidth, three schemes were compared for sound-level matching across bandwidth: component level, root-mean-square power and loudness. The calculation of the loudness match was based on the model reported by Moore and Glasberg [Acta Acust. 82, 335-345 (1996)]. Ten normally hearing volunteers were scanned using functional magnetic resonance imaging (tMRI) while listening to a 300-Hz tone presented at six different sound levels between 66 and 91 dB SPL and a harmonic-complex tone (F0= 186 Hz) presented at 65 and 85 dB SPL. This range of sound levels encompassed all three bases of sound-level matching. Activation in the superior temporal gyrus, induced by each of the eight tone conditions relative to a quiet baseline condition, was quantified as to extent and magnitude. Sound level had a small, but significant, effect on the extent of activation for the pure tone, but not for the harmonic-complex tone, while it had a significant effect on the response magnitude for both types of stimulus. Response magnitude increased linearly as a function of sound level for the full range of levels for the pure tone. The harmonic-complex tone produced greater activation than the pure tone, irrespective of the matching scheme for sound level, indicating that bandwidth had a greater effect on the pattern of auditory activation than sound level. Nevertheless, when the data were collapsed across stimulus class, extent and magnitude were significantly correlated with the loudness scale (measured in phons), but not with the intensity scale (measured in SPL). We therefore recommend the loudness formula as the most appropriate basis of matching sound level to control for loudness effects when cortical responses to other stimulus attributes, such as stimulus class, are the principal concern.  相似文献   

11.

Background  

Autonomous reflexes enable animals to respond quickly to potential threats, prevent injury and mediate fight or flight responses. Intense acoustic stimuli with sudden onsets elicit a startle reflex while stimuli of similar intensity but with longer rise times only cause a cardiac defence response. In laboratory settings, habituation appears to affect all of these reflexes so that the response amplitude generally decreases with repeated exposure to the stimulus. The startle reflex has become a model system for the study of the neural basis of simple learning processes and emotional processing and is often used as a diagnostic tool in medical applications. However, previous studies did not allow animals to avoid the stimulus and the evolutionary function and long-term behavioural consequences of repeated startling remain speculative. In this study we investigate the follow-up behaviour associated with the startle reflex in wild-captured animals using an experimental setup that allows individuals to exhibit avoidance behaviour.  相似文献   

12.
In order to determine the effects of hearing loss and spectral shaping on a dynamic spectral speech cue, behavioral identification and neural response patterns of stop-consonant stimuli varying along the /b-d-g/ place-of-articulation continuum were measured from 11 young adults (mean age = 27 years) and 10 older adults (mean age = 55.2 years) with normal hearing, and compared to those from 10 older adults (mean age = 61.3 years) with mild-to-moderate hearing impairment. Psychometric functions and N1-P2 cortical evoked responses were obtained using consonant-vowel (CV) stimuli with frequency-independent (unshaped) amplification as well as with frequency-dependent (shaped) amplification that enhanced F2 relative to the rest of the stimulus. Results indicated that behavioral identification and neural response patterns of stop-consonant CVs were affected primarily by aging and secondarily by age-related hearing loss. Further, enhancing the audibility of the F2 transition cue with spectrally shaped amplification partially reduced the effects of age-related hearing loss on categorization ability but not neural response patterns of stop-consonant CVs. These findings suggest that aging affects excitatory and inhibitory processes and may contribute to the perceptual differences of dynamic spectral cues seen in older versus young adults. Additionally, age and age-related hearing loss may have separate influences on neural function.  相似文献   

13.
Harmonic complexes comprised of the same spectral components in either positive-Schroeder (+Schr) or negative-Schroeder (-Schr) phase [see Schroeder, IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory 16, 85-89 (1970)] have identical long-term spectra and similar waveform envelopes. However, localized patterns of basilar-membrane (BM) excitation can be quite different in response to these two stimuli. Measurements in chinchillas showed more modulated (peakier) BM excitation for +Schr than -Schr complexes [Recio and Rhode, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108, 2281-2298 (2000)]. In the current study, laser velocimetry was used to examine BM responses at a location tuned to approximately 17 kHz in the basal turn of the guinea-pig cochlea, for +Schr and -Schr complexes with a 203-Hz fundamental frequency and including 101 equal-amplitude components from 2031 to 22,344 Hz. At 35-dB SPL, +Schr response waveforms showed greater amplitude modulation than -Schr responses. With increasing stimulation level, internal modulation decreased for both complexes. To understand the observed phenomena quantitatively, responses were predicted on the basis of a linearized model of the cochlea. Prediction was based on an "indirect impulse response" measured in the same animal. Response waveforms for Schroeder-phase signals were accurately predicted, provided that the level of the indirect impulse used in prediction closely matched the level of the Schroeder-phase stimulus. This result confirms that the underlying model, which originally was developed for noise stimuli, is valid for stimuli that produce completely different response waveforms. Moreover, it justifies explanation of cochlear filtering (i.e., differential treatment of different frequencies) in terms of a linear system.  相似文献   

14.
Vibratory measurements of the structures of the ear are key to understanding much of the pathology in mouse models of hearing loss. Unfortunately the high-speed sampling required to interrogate the high end of the mouse hearing spectrum is beyond the reach of most optical coherence tomography (OCT) systems. To address this issue, we have developed an algorithm that enables phase-sensitive OCT measurements over the full range of the mouse hearing spectrum (4-90 kHz). The algorithm phase-locks the line-trigger to the acoustic stimulation and then uses interleaved sampling to reconstruct the signal with higher temporal sampling. The algorithm was evaluated by measuring the vibratory response of mouse tympanic membrane to a pure tone stimulus.  相似文献   

15.
Under pure-tone stimulation, the spectrum of the period histogram recorded from primary auditory-nerve fibers at low and medium frequencies contains components at DC, at the applied tone frequency (the fundamental), and at a small number of harmonics of the tone frequency. The magnitudes and phases of these spectral components are examined. The spectral magnitudes of the fundamental and various harmonic components generally bear a fixed proportionality to each other over a broad range of signal conditions and nerve-fiber characteristics. This implies that the shape of the underlying rectified wave remains essentially unchanged over a broad range of stimulus intensities. For high-frequency stimuli, the fundamental and harmonic components are substantially attenuated. We provide a theoretical basis for the decrease of the spectralcomponent magnitudes with increasing harmonic number. For low-frequency pure-tone signals, the decrease is caused principally by the uncertainty in the position of neural-event occurrences within the half-wave-rectified period histogram. The lower the stimulus frequency, the greater this time uncertainty and therefore the lower the frequency at which the spectral components begin to diminish. For high-frequency pure-tone signals, on the other hand, the decrease is caused principally by the frequency rolloff associated with nervespike time jitter (it is then called loss of phase locking or loss of synchrony). Since some of this jitter arises from noise in the auditory nerve, it can be minimized by using peak detection rather than level detection. Using a specially designed microcomputer that measures the times at which the peaks of the action potentials occur, we have demonstrated the presence of phase locking to tone frequencies as high as 18 kHz. The traditional view that phase locking is always lost above 6 kHz is clearly not valid. This indicates that the placeversus-periodicity dichotomy in auditory theory requires reexaraination.  相似文献   

16.
We present results from a pilot study directed at developing an anchorable subjective speech quality test. The test uses multidimensional scaling techniques to obtain quantitative information about the perceptual attributes of speech. In the first phase of the study, subjects ranked perceptual distances between samples of speech produced by two different talkers, one male and one female, processed by a variety of codecs. The resulting distance matrices were processed to obtain, for each talker, a stimulus space for the various speech samples. This stimulus space has the properties that distances between stimuli in this space correspond to perceptual distances between stimuli and that the dimensions of this space correspond to attributes used by the subjects in determining perceptual distances. Mean opinion scores (MOS) scores obtained in an earlier study were found to be highly correlated with position in the stimulus space, and the three dimensions of the stimulus space were found to have identifiable physical and perceptual correlates. In the second phase of the study, we developed techniques for fitting speech generated by a new codec under investigation into a previously established stimulus space. The user is provided with a collection of speech samples and with the stimulus space for these speech samples as determined by a large-scale listening test. The user then carries out a much smaller listening test to determine the position of the new stimulus in the previously established stimulus space. This system is anchorable, so that different versions of a codec under development can be compared directly, and it provides more detailed information than the single number provided by MOS testing. We suggest that this information could be used to advantage in algorithm development and in development of objective measures of speech quality.  相似文献   

17.
Intensity difference limens (DLs) were measured in users of the Nucleus 22 and Clarion v1.2 cochlear implants and in normal-hearing listeners to better understand mechanisms of intensity discrimination in electric and acoustic hearing and to evaluate the possible role of neural adaptation. Intensity DLs were measured for three modes of presentation: gated (intensity increments gated synchronously with the pedestal), fringe (intensity increments delayed 250 or 650 ms relative to the onset of the pedestal), and continuous (intensity increments occur in the presence of a pedestal that is played throughout the experimental run). Stimuli for cochlear-implant listeners were trains of biphasic pulses; stimuli for normal-hearing listeners were a 1-kHz tone and a wideband noise. Clarion cochlear-implant listeners showed level-dependent effects of presentation mode. At low pedestal levels, gated thresholds were generally similar to thresholds obtained in the fringe and continuous conditions. At higher pedestal levels, however, the fringe and continuous conditions produced smaller intensity DLs than the gated condition, similar to the gated-continuous difference in intensity DLs observed in acoustic hearing. Nucleus cochlear-implant listeners did not show consistent threshold differences for the gated and fringe conditions, and were not tested in the continuous condition. It is not clear why a difference between gated and fringe thresholds occurred for the Clarion but not the Nucleus subjects. Normal-hearing listeners showed improved thresholds for the continuous condition relative to the gated condition, but the effect was larger for the 1-kHz tonal carrier than for the noise carrier. Findings suggest that adaptation occurring central to the inner hair cell synapse mediates the gated-continuous difference observed in Clarion cochlear-implant listeners and may also contribute to the gated-continuous difference in acoustic hearing.  相似文献   

18.
利用耳声发射原理,借鉴三间隔范式分离方法,提出对骨导振子施加扫频音,以快速准确获取全频段刺激频率耳声发射(SFOAE)信号的方法,以此研究真人头部的骨导效应,将测得的耳声信号和骨导振子激励信号之间的相对关系定义为耳声相关骨传导响应函数(OAR-BCRF)。10名听力正常的受试者的OAR-BCRF实验结果表明,不同刺激强度下测量的OAR-BCRF的包络形状差异不大,仅幅度随着刺激强度的增加而整体增大;乳突和下颌骨髁突两个不同刺激位置下的OAR-BCRF包络整体趋势相似,但在不同频段存在差异,下颌骨髁突处的经颅传输要低于乳突。受试者的平均OAR-BCRF数据显示,在2~6 kHz之间,同侧OAR-BCRF的幅度最大相差13 dB,而对侧OAR-BCRF最大相差19 dB。实验也发现同侧与对侧的OAR-BCRF包络相似且双侧骨传导的隔离度不高。本文的OAR-BCRF研究有效地探讨了真人头部的骨导传输的生理特性,可为经颅衰减和骨传导空间声定位等相关研究提供基础。  相似文献   

19.
It is now undisputed that the best frequency (BF) of basal basilar-membrane (BM) sites shifts downwards as the stimulus level increases. The direction of the shift for apical sites is, by contrast, less well established. Auditory nerve studies suggest that the BF shifts in opposite directions for apical and basal BM sites with increasing stimulus level. This study attempts to determine if this is the case in humans. Psychophysical tuning curves (PTCs) were measured using forward masking for probe frequencies of 125, 250, 500, and 6000 Hz. The level of a masker tone required to just mask a fixed low-level probe tone was measured for different masker-probe time intervals. The duration of the intervals was adjusted as necessary to obtain PTCs for the widest possible range of masker levels. The BF was identified from function fits to the measured PTCs and it almost always decreased with increasing level. This result is inconsistent with most auditory-nerve observations obtained from other mammals. Several explanations are discussed, including that it may be erroneous to assume that low-frequency PTCs reflect the tuning of apical BM sites exclusively and that the inherent frequency response of the inner hair cell may account for the discrepancy.  相似文献   

20.
Sound conditioning (pre-exposure to a moderate-level acoustic stimulus) can induce resistance to hearing loss from a subsequent traumatic exposure. Most sound conditioning experiments have utilized long-duration tones and noise at levels below 110 dB SPL as traumatic stimuli. It is important to know if sound conditioning can also provide protection from brief, high-level stimuli such as impulses produced by gunfire, and whether there are differences between females and males in the response of the ear to noise. In the present study, chinchillas were exposed to 95 dB SPL octave band noise centered at 0.5 kHz for 6 h/day for 5 days. After 5 days of recovery, they were exposed to simulated M16 rifle fire at a level of 150 dB peak SPL. Animals that were sound conditioned showed less hearing loss and smaller hair cell lesions than controls. Females showed significantly less hearing loss than males at low frequencies, but more hearing loss at 16 kHz. Cochleograms showed slightly less hair cell loss in females than in males. The results show that significant protection from impulse noise can be achieved with a 5-day conditioning regimen, and that there are consistent differences between female and male chinchillas in the response of the cochlea to impulse noise.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号