首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   32篇
  免费   0篇
化学   2篇
数学   2篇
物理学   28篇
  2019年   1篇
  2006年   1篇
  2005年   1篇
  2004年   1篇
  2002年   1篇
  2000年   2篇
  1999年   2篇
  1996年   3篇
  1994年   1篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   1篇
  1984年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1970年   3篇
  1969年   2篇
  1967年   1篇
排序方式: 共有32条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
Thresholds for the detection of the direction of travel of a moving sound source were determined in a single-interval, forced-choice paradigm. Both the rate at which the sound source is displaced (8 degrees-128 degrees/s) and the frequency of the signal to be localized (500-3700 Hz) affect dynamic spatial resolution. There is an inverse relationship between spatial resolution and the rate of travel, a finding that replicates an earlier observation on performance with sources displaced at high velocities [Perrott and Musicant, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 62, 1463-1466 (1977)]. However, the magnitude of this effect depends on the actual velocities employed. Relatively small changes in spatial resolution are apparent for velocities below approximately 32 degrees/s. The significant frequency effect can be summarized as follows: Dynamic spatial resolution is better for signals below 1000 Hz than for signals above this value (within the range tested). Particularly poor resolution is evident for signals between 1300-2000 Hz. The present results indicate that signal frequency affects dynamic spatial resolution in a fashion similar to that which has been observed in the more common "static" localization test situation. There is no indication of an interaction between these two variables. These results provide additional support for the hypothesis that both static and dynamic spatial discrimination functions are dependent upon the same underlying mechanisms. The effects of velocity upon the spatial resolution problem, a unique aspect of the dynamic paradigm, can probably be explained without the necessity of additional hypothetical mechanisms in the auditory system (e.g., a specialized motion detector).  相似文献   
2.
3.
Minimum audible movement angles as a function of sound source trajectory   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Auditory resolution of moving sound sources was determined in a simulated motion paradigm for sources traveling along horizontal, vertical, or oblique orientations in the subjects's frontal plane. With motion restricted to the horizontal orientation, minimum audible movement angles (MAMA) ranged from about 1.7 degrees at the lowest velocity (1.8 degrees/s) to roughly 10 degrees at the highest velocity (320 degrees/s). With the sound moving along an oblique orientation (rotated 45 degrees relative to the horizontal) MAMAs generally matched those of the horizontal condition. When motion was restricted to the vertical, MAMAs were substantially larger at all velocities (often exceeding 8 degrees). Subsequent tests indicated that MAMAs are a U-shaped function of velocity, with optimum resolution obtained at about 2 degrees/s for the horizontal (and oblique) and 7-11 degrees/s for the vertical orientation. Additional tests conducted at a fixed velocity of 1.8 degrees/s along oblique orientations of 80 degrees and 87 degrees indicated that even a small deviation from the vertical had a significant impact on MAMAs. A displacement of 10 degrees from the vertical orientation (a slope of 80 degrees) was sufficient to reduce thresholds (obtained at a velocity of 1.8 degrees/s) from about 11 degrees to approximately 2 degrees (a fivefold increase in acuity). These results are in good agreement with our previous study of minimum audible angles long oblique planes [Perrott and Saberi, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 87, 1728-1731 (1990)].(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   
4.
This study examines the difficulties college students experience when creating and interpreting graphs in which speed is one of the variables. Nineteen students, all preservice elementary or middle school teachers, completed an upper‐level course exploring algebraic concepts. Although all of these preservice teachers had previously completed several mathematics courses, including calculus, they demonstrated widespread misconceptions about the variable speed. This study identifies four cognitive obstacles held by the students, provides excerpts of their graphical constructions and verbal interpretations, and discusses potential causes for the confusion. In particular, misconceptions arose when students interpreted the behavior and nature of speed within a graphical context, as well as in situations where they were required to construct a graph involving speed as a variable. The study concludes by offering implications for the teaching and learning of speed and its interpretation within a graphical setting.  相似文献   
5.
In the first experiment, subjects were asked to discriminate whether a sound was emanating from a moving or stationary source. The minimum audible movement angle (MAMA) thus defined was observed to increase as the source velocity increased. MAMA ranged from a low of 8.3 degrees with the slowest velocity employed (90 degrees/s) to a high of 21.2 degrees with the fastest velocity (360 degrees/s). In the second experiment, subjects were asked to localize where the moving source was, at signal on and offset. The results indicate that the apparent onset is displaced in the direction of motion and the amount of this displacement is directly related to source velocity. Less consistent results were observed with signal offset. The present results suggest that the binaural system is relatively insensitive to motion.  相似文献   
6.
Fast atom bombardment, combined with high-energy collision-induced tandem mass spectrometry, has been used to investigate gas-phase metal-ion interactions with captopril, enalaprilat and lisinopril, all angiotensin-converting enzyme inhibitors.Suggestions for the location of metal-binding sites are presented. For captopril, metal binding occurs most likely at both the sulphur and the nitrogen atom. For enalaprilat and lisinopril, binding preferably occurs at the amine nitrogen. Copyright 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
7.
Interaural differences of time (IDT) thresholds were measured with 600-microseconds transients. The initial experiment was a successful replication of previous experiments that have obtained the precedence effect in lateralization paradigms (e.g., Yost and Soderquist, 1984). When a dichotic click followed a diotic click with an interclick interval (ICI) less than 1 ms or larger than 5 ms, IDT thresholds were generally less than 40 microseconds. For ICIs between 1 to 5 ms, IDT thresholds increased to approximately 220 microseconds. Poorest performance was observed for ICIs of 1.75 to 2.35 ms. During the course of conducting a series of planned experiments on this effect, a substantial drop in IDT thresholds was observed across the ICIs of maximum interest (1 to 5 ms). The precedence effect, which we had replicated in our initial experiment, essentially "disappeared" when the subjects were given sufficient practice on the lateralization task. A number of conditions were explored in an unsuccessful attempt to recover the precedence effect in these experienced subjects. The implications of these results are discussed.  相似文献   
8.
Two experiments were conducted to examine the ability of human listeners to localize the "lag" or "echo" source in a precedence effect paradigm. A 5-ms noise burst was presented from a source located between 554-279 cm from the subject. This "lead" source was always located at 0 degrees azimuth. At the same time, one of two sources located at a distance of 610 cm from the subject was also activated with the same 5-ms noise burst. The subject's task was to identify which lag source had been active. Across sessions, the angular distance between the lag sources was varied, so as to allow a determination of the minimum audible angle (MAA) that could be resolved. Tests were run in a room designed to minimize reflections and in a hallway that was acoustically quite complex. No systematic differences in MAA thresholds were observed as a function of the environment employed. MAA thresholds obtained without the signal from the lead speaker were less than 1 degree for four of the five subjects tested. The precedence effect, as measured by the change in the MAA threshold, appears to have only a modest influence on localization performance. Under conditions in which the lead source was concurrently active, the thresholds were generally elevated by only 2 degrees-4 degrees. A reduction of this magnitude in the ability to resolve the position of the lag source does not seem to be sufficient, in itself, to account for the excellent localization performance frequently observed in reflective environments.  相似文献   
9.
10.
Thin films of TiO2 (anatase) nanoparticles are assembled at an electrode surface via a layer-by-layer deposition process employing phytic acid, pyromellitic acid, or flavin adenine dinucleotide (FAD) as molecular binders. With all three types of binders, layers of typically 30 nm thickness are formed each deposition cycle. FAD as an electrochemically active component immobilized at the surface of the TiO2 particles is reduced to FADH2 and reoxidized in a chemically reversible two electron-two proton redox process. Two distinct voltammetric signals are observed for the immobilized FAD redox system associated with (i) hopping of electrons at the TiO2 surface (reversible) and (ii) conduction of electrons through the TiO2 assembly (irreversible). The conduction of electrons through the TiO2 assembly is possible by diffusion over considerable distances as well as through a "spacer" layer of TiO2 phytate. An order of magnitude (upper limit) estimate for the diffusion coefficient of electrons through TiO2 phytate, D(electron) approximately 10(-6) m(2) s(-1), is obtained from voltammetric data. Finally, it is demonstrated that the calcination of TiO2 assemblies causes dramatic changes in the electron transfer kinetics for the immobilized FAD/FADH2 redox system.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号