首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 98 毫秒
1.
Intensity difference limens (DLs) were measured over a wide intensity range for 200-ms, 1-kHz gated tones and for 200-ms increments in continuous 1-kHz tones. Magnitude estimates also were obtained for the gated tones over a comparable intensity range. The discrimination data are in general agreement with those from earlier studies but they extend them by showing: (1) good discrimination for gated tones over at least a 115-dB dynamic range; (2) a slight increase in the relative DL (delta I/I) as intensity increases above 95 dB SPL; (3) smaller DLs for increments than for gated tones, with the difference approximately independent of intensity; (4) negligible "negative masking" when thresholds are expressed as intensity differences (delta I). For two of the three subjects, magnitude estimates do not conform to a single-exponent power law for suprathreshold intensities. Over the middle range of intensities where a single exponent is appropriate, the value of the exponent is less than 0.1 for all subjects.  相似文献   

2.
Two experiments investigated the temporal integration of trains of tone pulses by normal and by cochlearly impaired listeners. In the first experiment, thresholds were measured for a single 5-ms, 4-kHz tone pulse, and for ten such tone pulses as a function of interpulse interval (delta t). For normal listeners, temporal integration, defined as the threshold difference between one and ten pulses, was about 8 dB for delta t less than 20 ms, and about 5 dB at longer delta t's. For impaired listeners, temporal integration was only about 2-3 dB across the range of delta t's (5-160 ms) studied. A second experiment measured psychometric functions (log d' versus log signal power) for a single pulse and for ten pulses with delta t's of 5 ms and 80 ms. The normal listeners' functions had slopes close to unity in all three conditions, with a few exceptions. The impaired listeners' functions had slopes close to unity for ten pulses with delta t = 5 ms, but had slopes significantly greater than unity for delta t = 80 ms, and for a single pulse. At delta t = 80 ms, the increase in d' relative to the condition with a single tone was similar (a factor of square root of 10) for both impaired and normal listeners, but the threshold difference was smaller for the impaired listeners due to their steeper psychometric functions. For impaired listeners, then, temporal integration at delta t = 80 ms was normal in terms of a change in d' but abnormal when measured as a threshold difference.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

3.
There is disagreement among theorists over the exact measure to be used to quantify auditory level discrimination. It has been proposed that, for level discrimination tasks, the measure that is most linearly related to the sensitivity index, d', will be the correct measure. The level difference (deltaL) and the Weber fraction (theta) are both candidates, though the latter is sensitive to the physical unit in which it is expressed (e.g., pressure or intensity) while the former is not. Psychometric functions for level discrimination were obtained at a number of pedestal levels for 10-ms sinusoids (either 1000 or 6500 Hz) and broadband noise bursts. These functions were used to assess which of three measures: deltaL, theta = deltap/p, or theta = deltaI/I, is most nearly linearly related to d'. The results suggest that deltap/p is the measure that comes closest to being linearly related to d'.  相似文献   

4.
Capacities of the goldfish for intensity discrimination were studied using classical respiratory conditioning and a staircase psychophysical procedure. Physiological studies on single saccular (auditory) nerve fibers under similar stimulus conditions helped characterize the dimensions of neural activity used in intensity discrimination. Incremental intensity difference limens (IDLs in dB) for 160-ms increments in continuous noise, 500-ms noise bursts, and 500-ms, 800-Hz tone bursts are 2 to 3 dB, are independent of overall level, and vary with signal duration according to a power function with a slope averaging - 0.33. Noise decrements are relatively poorly detected and the silent gap detection threshold is about 35 ms. The IDLs for increments and decrements in an 800-Hz continuous tone are about 0.13 dB, are independent of duration, and are level dependent. Unlike mammalian auditory nerve fibers, some goldfish saccular fibers show variation in recovery time to tonal increments and decrements, and adaptation to a zero rate. Unit responses to tone increments and decrements show rate effects generally in accord with previous observations on intracellular epsp's in goldfish saccular fibers. Neurophysiological correlates of psychophysical intensity discrimination data suggest the following: (1) noise gap detection may be based on spike rate increments which follow gap offset; (2) detection of increments and decrements in continuous tones may be determined by steep low-pass filtering in peripheral neural channels which enhance the effects of spectral "splatter" toward the lower frequencies; (3) IDLs for pulsed signals of different duration can be predicted from the slopes of rate-intensity functions and spike rate variability in individual auditory nerve fibers; and (4) at different sound pressure levels, different populations of peripheral fibers provide the information used in intensity discrimination.  相似文献   

5.
The form of the psychometric function (PF) for auditory frequency discrimination is of theoretical interest and practical importance. In this study, PFs for pure-tone frequency discrimination were measured for several standard frequencies (200-8000 Hz) and levels [35-85 dB sound pressure level (SPL)] in normal-hearing listeners. The proportion-correct data were fitted using a cumulative-Gaussian function of the sensitivity index, d', computed as a power transformation of the frequency difference, Δf. The exponent of the power function corresponded to the slope of the PF on log(d')-log(Δf) coordinates. The influence of attentional lapses on PF-slope estimates was investigated. When attentional lapses were not taken into account, the estimated PF slopes on log(d')-log(Δf) coordinates were found to be significantly lower than 1, suggesting a nonlinear relationship between d' and Δf. However, when lapse rate was included as a free parameter in the fits, PF slopes were found not to differ significantly from 1, consistent with a linear relationship between d' and Δf. This was the case across the wide ranges of frequencies and levels tested in this study. Therefore, spectral and temporal models of frequency discrimination must account for a linear relationship between d' and Δf across a wide range of frequencies and levels.  相似文献   

6.
Detection and intensity discrimination of a sinusoid   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Intensity discrimination thresholds were measured for gated 100-ms, 1000-Hz tones. Discrimination thresholds were measured at several intensities near absolute threshold as well as at 30, 60, and 90 dB SPL. Psychometric functions were obtained for several of these discrimination conditions, and for detection of the signal in quiet. The results showed that Weber's law is approximately valid for standards as low as 0 dB SL. Small amounts of negative masking were observed even when the data were expressed in terms of increment energy. The psychometric functions for the discrimination conditions had a similar form and were shallower than the psychometric function for the detection of a signal in quiet. A similar set of conditions was run in the presence of a continuous, broadband noise. The results were generally in agreement with those obtained in quiet, but slight differences suggested that the variability which limits performance in the two conditions is different. The results are discussed in terms of the effects of nonlinear transduction, the effects of uncertainty, and contrast mechanisms as proposed by Laming [Sensory Analysis (Academic, London, 1986)].  相似文献   

7.
Level discrimination as a function of level for tones from 0.25 to 16 kHz   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Difference limens for level (delta L in dB = 20 log [(p + delta p)/p], where p is pressure) were measured as a function of level for tones at 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, 4, 8, 10, 12, 14, and 16 kHz. At each frequency, test levels encompassed the range from near threshold to 95 dB SPL in steps of 10 dB or smaller. The stimulus duration was 500 ms and the interstimulus interval was 250 ms. An adaptive two-alternative forced-choice procedure with feedback was used. Results for six normal listeners show individual differences among listeners, but the general trends seen in the average data clearly are present in the individual data and show the following. First, the delta Ls at all but the highest frequencies are generally smaller at high levels than at low levels. Second, the delta Ls at equal SPLs are largely independent of frequency up to about 4 kHz, but increase with frequency above 4 kHz. Third, at 8 and 10 kHz, the delta Ls are clearly nonmonotonic functions of level, showing consistent deterioration in the mid-level delta Ls relative to the low- and high-level delta Ls. The present data are discussed qualitatively in terms of current models of level discrimination.  相似文献   

8.
The intensity jnd is often assumed to depend on the slope of the loudness function. One way to test this assumption is to measure the jnd for a sound that falls on distinctly different loudness functions. Two such functions were generated by presenting a 1000-Hz tone in narrow-band noise (925-1080 Hz) set at 70 dB SPL and in wideband noise (75-9600 Hz) set at 80 dB SPL. Over a range from near threshold to about 75 dB SPL, the loudness function for the tone is much steeper in the narrow-band noise than in the wideband noise. At 72 dB SPL, where the two loudness curves cross, the tone's jnd was measured in each noise by a block up-down two-interval forced-choice procedure. Despite the differences in slope (and in sensation level), the jnd (delta I/I) is nearly the same in the two noises, 0.22 in narrow-band noise and 0.20 in wideband noise. The mean value of 0.21 is close to the value of 0.25 interpolated from Jesteadt et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 61, 169-176 (1977)] for a 1000-Hz tone that had the same loudness in quiet as did our 72-dB tone in noise, but lay on a loudness function with a much lower slope. These and other data demonstrate that intensity discrimination for pure tones is unrelated to the slope of the loudness function.  相似文献   

9.
Frequency discrimination in the monkey   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
This study evaluated frequency discrimination ability in 11 monkeys over an extended period of time using a repeating-standard procedure and the method of constant stimuli. The intersubject variability of the difference limens for frequency (delta F) was large, as reported by other investigators, but similar in magnitude to the variability of the difference limens for intensity (delta I) from three of the same subjects in an intensity discrimination experiment. Continued training generally resulted in a rapid decrease in delta F's, followed by a longer-term, slower decrease. For one subject delta F's slowly decreased throughout a 190-week time period. This long-term training effect was specific to frequency discrimination; a similar effect was not observed for the same subject tested in an intensity discrimination experiment. Finally, delta F's from the well-trained monkeys of this study were larger than monkey delta F's from this laboratory reported in an earlier study, and than human delta F's. An anatomical explanation for the human/monkey delta F magnitude difference is explored.  相似文献   

10.
This paper examines how the difference limen for level, delta L, is affected by stimulus bandwidth and variability. The delta L's were measured in three normal listeners using an adaptive two-interval, forced-choice procedure. The 30-ms stimuli were a 3-kHz tone and nine noise bands with half-power bandwidths ranging from 50 Hz-12 kHz. Except for the 12-kHz bandwidth, which was a low-pass noise, the noise bands were centered at 3 kHz. The delta L's were measured for both frozen and random noises presented at 30, 60, or 90 dB SPL overall. For frozen noises, the same sample of noise was presented throughout a block of 50 trials; for the random noises, different samples of noise were used in each interval of the trials. Results show that the delta L's are higher for random than for frozen noises at narrow bandwidths, but not at wide bandwidths. The delta L's for frozen narrow-band noises decrease with increasing level and are similar to those for the pure tone, whereas the delta L's for wideband noises are only slightly smaller at 90 than at 30 dB SPL. An unexpected finding is that the delta L's are larger at 60 than at 30 dB SPL for both frozen and random noises with bandwidths greater than one critical band. The effect of bandwidth varies with level: The delta L's decrease with increasing bandwidth at low levels, but are nearly independent of bandwidth at 90 dB SPL. The interaction of bandwidth and level is consistent with the multiband excitation-pattern model, but the nonmonotonic behavior of delta L as a function of level suggests modifications to the model.  相似文献   

11.
A positive reinforcement conditioning procedure was used to train chinchillas to respond to intensity differences between successively occurring tone bursts. Intensity difference limens were measured at 0.5, 1, 4, and 8 kHz at five intensities ranging from 10- to 55-dB sensation level. The intensity difference limen decreased from approximately 8 dB near threshold to approximately 3.5 dB at the highest level. The intensity difference limens for the chinchilla were considerably larger than those for humans as well as several other mammals; however, the results were similar to those obtained for the parakeet. The present results from intensity discrimination appeared to be related to previous data for the discrimination of amplitude modulated noise.  相似文献   

12.
Lateralization and frequency selectivity in normal and impaired hearing   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The onset-time difference delta T required to lateralize a 30-ms bifrequency tone burst toward the leading ear was measured as a function of the frequency difference delta F between the tone in the left ear and the tone in the right ear. At center frequencies of 0.5 and 4 kHz, four normal listeners tested at 80 and 100 dB SPL had delta Ts that were relatively constant at subcritical delta Fs, but increased at delta Fs wider than a critical band. At 1 kHz, delta T increased with delta F even at subcritical delta Fs. Ten listeners with cochlear impairments were tested at 100 dB SPL. Seven had normal delta Ts at 4 kHz, despite hearing losses between 50 and 70 dB. At 0.5 and 1 kHz, mildly impaired listeners had nearly normal lateralization functions, whereas more severely imparied listeners had very large delta Ts and no frequency selectivity. These and other findings indicate that listeners even with moderate to severe hearing losses can lateralize normally on the basis of interaural differences in onset envelope, but not on the basis of temporal differences in the fine structure.  相似文献   

13.
Level discrimination of tones as a function of duration   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Difference limens for level [delta Ls (dB) = 20 log[p + delta p)/p), where p is the pressure] were measured as a function of duration for tones at 250, 500, and 8000 Hz. Stimulus durations ranged from 2 ms to 2 s, and the stimulus power was held constant. Rise and fall times were 1 ms. The interstimulus interval was 250 ms. At each frequency, three levels were tested: 85, 65, and approximately 40 dB SPL. An adaptive two-alternative forced-choice procedure with feedback was used. For three normal listeners, delta Ls decreased as duration increased, up to at least 2 s, except at 250 Hz. At 250 Hz, delta L stopped decreasing at durations between 0.5 and 1 s. In a double logarithmic plot of delta L versus duration, the rate of decrease is generally well fitted by a sloping line. The average slope is -0.28; it is steeper at high levels than at low levels. Because the average slope is shallower than the -0.5 slope predicted for an optimum detector, it may be that fast adaptation of auditory-nerve activity and/or memory effects interfere with level discrimination of long-duration tones. Finally, the delta Ls at 8 kHz decreased nonmonotonically with increasing level.  相似文献   

14.
The effects of forward masker duration on psychophysical measures of frequency selectivity were investigated in two experiments. In both experiments, masker duration was 50 or 400 ms, signal duration was 20 ms, and there was no delay between masker offset and signal onset. In the first experiment, growth-of-masking functions were measured for a masker whose frequency was below, at, or above the 1000-Hz signal frequency. From those data, input filter patterns (IFPs) were plotted for masker levels from 40-90 dB SPL. In the second experiment, masking patterns (MPs) were measured for a 1000-Hz masker presented at 50, 70, and 90 dB SPL. Both measures of frequency selectivity (IFPs and MPs) indicate that frequency selectivity is greater for the 400-ms masker. These data suggest that there may be a sharpening of frequency selectivity with time at a stage prior to the adaptation observed in forward masking.  相似文献   

15.
Intensity-discrimination thresholds were measured for a 25-ms, 6-kHz pure tone for pedestal levels from 40 to 90 dB sound pressure level (SPL) with and without a forward masker (100-ms narrowband Gaussian noise, N(0)=70 dB). When the masker was present, the masker and probe were separated by 100 ms of silence. Unmasked and masked thresholds were measured in a two-interval monaural procedure and, separately, in a single-interval interaural procedure in which the pedestal and incremented pedestals were presented simultaneously to opposite ears. While the monaural thresholds were elevated markedly by the forward masker for mid-level pedestals, interaural thresholds were nearly unaffected by the masker across pedestal levels. The results argue against the notion that the monaural elevation in forward-masked thresholds is due to degraded encoding of intensity information at early stages of auditory processing.  相似文献   

16.
Psychometric functions (PFs) for forward-masked tones were obtained for conditions in which signal level was varied to estimate threshold at several masker levels (variable-signal condition), and in which masker level was varied to estimate threshold at several signal levels (variable-masker condition). The changes in PF slope across combinations of masker frequency, masker level, and signal delay were explored in three experiments. In experiment 1, a 2-kHz, 10-ms tone was masked by a 50, 70 or 90 dB SPL, 20-ms on-frequency forward masker, with signal delays of 2, 20, or 40 ms, in a variable-signal condition. PF slopes decreased in conditions where signal threshold was high. In experiments 2 and 3, the signal was a 4-kHz, 10-ms tone, and the masker was either a 4- or 2.4-kHz, 200-ms tone. In experiment 2, on-frequency maskers were presented at 30 to 90 dB SPL in 10-dB steps and off-frequency maskers were presented at 60 to 90 dB SPL in 10-dB steps, with signal delays of 0, 10, or 30 ms, in a variable-signal condition. PF slopes decreased as signal level increased, and this trend was similar for on- and off-frequency maskers. In experiment 3, variable-masker conditions with on- and off-frequency maskers and 0-ms signal delay were presented. In general, the results were consistent with the hypothesis that peripheral nonlinearity is reflected in the PF slopes. The data also indicate that masker level plays a role independent of signal level, an effect that could be accounted for by assuming greater internal noise at higher stimulus levels.  相似文献   

17.
Psychophysical forward-masked thresholds were estimated for 3- and 6-month-old infants and for adults. Listeners detected a repeated 1000-Hz probe, with 16-ms rise time, no steady-state duration, and 16-ms fall time. Unmasked thresholds were determined for one group of listeners who were trained to respond when they heard the probe but not at other times. In the masking conditions, each tone burst was preceded by a 100-ms broadband noise masker at 65 dB SPL. Listeners were trained to respond when they heard the probe and masker, but not when they heard the masker alone. The masker-probe interval, delta t, was either 5, 10, 25, or 200 ms. Four groups of subjects listened in the masked conditions, each at one value of delta t. Each listener attempted to complete a block of 32 trials including four probe levels chosen to span the range of expected thresholds. "Group" thresholds, based on average psychometric functions, as well as thresholds for individual listeners, were estimated. Both group and individual thresholds declined with delta t, as expected, for both infants and adults. Infants' masked thresholds were higher than those of adults, and comparison of masked to unmasked thresholds suggested that infants demonstrate more forward masking than adults, particularly at short delta t. Forward masking appeared to have greater effects on 3-month-olds' detection than on either 6-month-olds' or adults'. Compared to adults, 6-month-olds demonstrated more forward masking only for delta t of 5 ms. Thus, susceptibility to forward masking may be nearly mature by 6 months of age.  相似文献   

18.
The observer was asked to judge whether a comparison sound was more or less intense than a standard sound of 60 dB SPL. The sounds were broadband noises of 100-ms duration. The independent variables were the range of intensities used in the set of comparison sounds and the presence or absence of feedback. The main dependent variable was the variability of such judgments, which was measured from the psychometric function. The variability, measured in terms of the Weber function, increased from about 1.5 to 4 dB when the stimulus range changed from 10 to 60 dB. Increases in stimulus range increased the variability of the binary judgments in this task, as they had in previous experiments where multiple responses were required. Feedback improved performance primarily at the largest ranges. Only a small fraction of these changes in variability stems from sequential effects.  相似文献   

19.
Three monaural chinchillas were trained to detect intensity decrements in broadband noise (20 kHz) using a shock-avoidance conditioning procedure. The intensity decrements were presented at one of nine different durations between 2 and 35 ms at noise levels of 25, 45, and 65 dB SPL. At each intensity-duration combination, the level of the decrement was varied to obtain a decrement threshold. The minimal detectable decrement decreased from approximately 20 dB at the shortest duration to an asymptote of roughly 4 dB at approximately 30 ms. The data were modeled by a low-pass filter with an 11-ms time constant. The decrement detection function of the chinchilla is similar to that of humans. However, long-duration decrement thresholds are larger in the chinchilla, as would be predicted from the large intensity difference limen of the chinchilla. In general, there was little change in the decrement function across background intensities except that 2-ms decrements were not detected at the 25-dB SPL background intensity.  相似文献   

20.
Thresholds for amplitude modulation detection were obtained from four subjects at frequencies of 2, 4, 6, 8, and 10 kHz for sensation levels of 15, 30, 45, and 60 dB and modulation rates of 2, 4, and 8 Hz. High-frequency difference limens calculated from amplitude modulation thresholds were found to change nonmonotonically as a function of sensation level, independent of modulation rate. This nonmonotonic relation stemmed mainly from a gradual reduction of the difference limen at the lowest sensation level with increasing frequency. Difference limens for pulsed tone discrimination were also measured in two of the subjects at 2, 6, and 10 kHz and sensation levels of 15, 30, 45, and 60 dB. The relation between intensity discrimination and sensation level was similar to that found for amplitude modulation detection. These findings are interpreted as indicating that the nonmonotonic relation between sensation level and intensity resolution is a general characteristic of stimulus processing at higher frequencies.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号